Consider the two journal articles attached. Then, put together a 2 to 3-page reflection that discuss characteristics that might increase the risk of experiencing trauma. For example, consider how different populations may be at greater risk for experiencing trauma or how trauma might occur in on-campus, online, after-school, or community settings. Think about how trauma risk might be elevated for students and educators.
2-page reflection
Include three scholarly sources in addition to the attachment.
European Journal of Educational Sciences, Special Edition, October 2019, ISSN: 1857- 6036
76
Chronic Poverty: The Implications of Bullying,
Trauma, and the Education of the Poverty-Stricken
Population
Kevin Johnson, M.Ed.
Liberty University, United States
Doi: 10.19044/ejes.s.v6a6 URL:http://dx.doi.org/10.19044/ejes.s.v6a6
Abstract
Chronic poverty is a worldwide epidemic, and communities must take
a proactive approach to assist the poor by extending a hand to lift them up and
not hold them down. Tribulations are part of life, but are some afflictions self-
imposed, escalated, or reinforced by living in deprived contextual
environments. Poverty-stricken people experience more trauma throughout
their lifetime; they are less educated than their counterpart, causing them to
become targets in school, increasing their chances of being bullied and
demoralized. Bullying is not a rite of passage, and it has lifelong effects that
reveal itself in adulthood by strengthening generational curses, oppressing
families and communities, expanding the educational gap, and reinforcing the
cycle of chronic
poverty.
The research depicted in this article explores the
correlation between poverty, human development, trauma, pedagogical
implications, and bullying, characterizing the detrimental ramifications in
adulthood. The paper analyzes bully symptomology, the etiology of traumatic
experiences, and how the consequences of chronic poverty affect human
development that expands the educational gap between minorities and white
students. Trauma-focused cognitive behavioral therapy is an effective
empirically-based treatment modality to combat the symptoms of
posttraumatic stress. School systems must do a better job of educating
traumatized children living in poverty. The research ventures to explain
chronic poverty’s role in human development, traumatology, and education,
taking an inclusive approach to providing solutions to create a cultural shift
that will change the contextual environment and propel people to become self-
sufficient, more educated, and equipped to break the generational curse of
chronic poverty.
Keywords: Chronic poverty, trauma, bullying, CPTSD, education.
http://dx.doi.org/10.19044/ejes.s.v6a6
European Journal of Educational Sciences, Special Edition, October 2019, ISSN: 1857- 6036
77
Introduction:
President Thomas Jefferson wrote, “We hold these truths to be self-
evident, that all men are created equal, that they are endowed by their Creator
with certain unalienable Rights, that among these are life, liberty and the
pursuit of happiness.” In the eyes of their Creator, all men are created equal –
in the image of God, but throughout history, a man divided equality and justice
for all during slavery, Jim Crow, and the Civil Rights Movement. Many people
are miseducated and do not comprehend that the Civil Rights Movement was
not just for the Black race but all people, nationalities, ethnicities, gender, and
the disabled; the fight was for fairness for every color and creed. During this
period, the mentally challenged and disabled had struggles and battles of their
own. Fortunate enough, there were advocate groups and parents who were
willing to stand up and fight for the equal rights of those who could not stand
and fight for themselves. The mentally challenged and disabled have been
marginalized from society and the classrooms for years. Is it possible for
people who are reared in chronic poverty to beat the odds and succeed when
all odds are stacked against them? How is it possible to provide quality
education and security when children living in poverty normally stand out
from the crowd due to hunger, lack of resources, and poor hygiene, not
adequately having clothing, shelter, and other basic needs?
In President John F. Kennedy’s inaugural address, he proclaimed, “And
so, my fellow Americans: ask not what your country can do for you—ask what
you can do for your country.” What happens when the country placed certain
people at a significant disadvantage by being born the wrong color? How can
those who live a life of chronic poverty and generational curses of destitute do
anything for their country when they lack education and financial resources?
Chronic poverty orchestrates a life of injustice, discrimination, prejudice, and
unequal opportunities, regardless of the misconception that people are created
equal. Chronic poverty puts people at higher risk of poor mental and physical
health, risky behaviors, substance use disorders, complex posttraumatic stress,
and live a traumatic and detrimental lifestyle that stifles education, religious
beliefs, maturity, and personal growth, which has the propensity to bind
individuals mentally, spiritually, and emotionally. Lifetime adversities and
trauma correlate with low self-esteem, mental illness, lower socioeconomic
status, and small educational achievement, especially in the Black and Latino
populations who happen to be underrepresented to most studies. Myers,
Wyatt, Ullman, Loeb, Chin, Prause, Zhang, Williams, Slavich, and Liu (2015)
asserted that minorities experience unique stressors that impact their health –
including stressors such as neglect by society, discrimination, and multiple
traumatic experiences. Hardships in life devitalize people, resulting in pain,
suffering, and stress, while others who perceived the same traumatization rally
up the power to not only survive but thrive.
European Journal of Educational Sciences, Special Edition, October 2019, ISSN: 1857- 6036
78
Generational poverty poses a distinct predisposition to the etiology of
the essence of traumatization because poverty can be a “little leaven” that
levels an entire generation. As the root cause of many traumatic events,
poverty has the proclivity to construct a unique dynamic that makes trauma
challenging to isolate and identify as a tragedy because families are described
as dysfunctional. Lowdermilk and Brunache (2013) professed that many
children reared in poverty mirrors the same behaviors as parents and their
community. They reflect those individuals growing up in demanding
environments, reporting negative/absent parent, parents with multiple sexual
partners or engaged in prostitution, substance use disorder, violence in the
home and community, verbal/physical abuse, unsanitary conditions, lack of
finances and resources, and parents demonstrate very low educational
expectations for their children, and all of this happens within the children’s
nuclear family. With the dismantling of the family structure, is it possible to
close the achievement gap and break the generational curse of the chronicity
of poverty?
Chronic Poverty and Human Development
Researchers examine the psychological and emotional development of
children living in poverty based on parental guidance and the methods parents
use to rear their children. Evans and Kim (2012) reported that “in the last two
decades, many scholars have investigated the underlying psychological
processes that explain why childhood poverty has such pervasive ill effects on
human development” (p. 43). Evidence substantiates that poverty causes
chronic stress, which deteriorates human functioning in every faculty of the
body, especially memory, brain development, and emotional regulation.
Children in poverty face unusual stressors that children living in middle-class
do not know about because they live in a contextual environment perceived as
safe and nurturing. Amatea and West (2007) declared that children living in
poverty are considerably more likely than children from the middle-class to
report increased levels of anxiety and depression, exhibiting a higher
frequency of behavioral and academic difficulties, and a lower level of
positive academic interaction and engagement in the school system. Collins,
Connors, Donohue, Gardner, Goldblatt, Hayward, Kiser, Strieder, and
Thompson (2010) reported that children who grow up in urban poverty display
symptoms of complex posttraumatic stress disorder; “complex trauma is a
varied and multifaceted phenomenon, frequently embedded in a matrix of
other psychosocial problems (e.g., neglect, marital discord, and domestic
violence that carry ongoing threat)” (p. 12). As reported by Gabrielli, Gill,
Koester, and Borntrager (2013), it takes a thorough understanding of the
ramifications of trauma and the unfolding developmental processes of
childhood as it is experienced in a particular culture, without neglecting to
European Journal of Educational Sciences, Special Edition, October 2019, ISSN: 1857- 6036
79
understand the nature of historical trauma, as aggregated spiritual, physical,
emotional, and psychological wounding over the lifespan and across
generations.
Studies show that the ability to sense touch occurs around eight weeks
of gestation, and fetal development of the other sensory organs begin around
five weeks of conception and continue to refine through birth and early
childhood (Uhernik, 2017). While in utero, the fetus can hear and respond to
sounds and noises outside of the womb. Many parents play music, read books,
and talk to their fetuses throughout the pregnancy, interacting by touching
certain spots of the mother’s belly by poking or grabbing a hand or foot when
it is visible. At birth, the newborn can only see eight to ten inches, and research
denotes this short distance is “precisely the range required for gazing into the
caregiver’s eyes when held and for beginning the visual and interactive dance
of attachment and bonding” (Uhernik, 2017, p. 54). Establishing a bond and
attachment between the mother and the baby is the first step in psychosocial
development, which is vital for successful progression throughout life. As
reported by Kim, Fonagy, Allen, and Strathearn (2014),
Maternal sensitivity to infant distress and non-distress as predictors of
infant-mother attachment security. Maternal attachment trauma, particularly
when unresolved, presents to the mother’s attunement to and management of
her infant’s distress. Although this has not yet been the subject of direct
empirical scrutiny, attachment researchers have long speculated that infants’
distress signals may activate unresolved traumatic memories in their
respective mothers, thereby initiating a cascade of compromised maternal
responses. (p. 354)
The maternal reactions manifest traumatic memories for the mother
and can affect memory and cognitive abilities for the newborn.
Erik Erikson was a renowned psychologist and anthropologist who
identified eight stages of psychosocial stages of development, from birth to
old age. Everyone must discover her sense of regulation as she interacts with
the environment and the biological, emotional, and psychological
idiosyncrasies in life. Infants must establish trust so that they feel safe in
knowing that their needs will be met, and they will not be neglected. Stability
and a sense of security allow infants to see the world as secure and a
dependable place, encouraging optimism about the future and having
confidence in themselves and other people. Mounting research postulates that
fetuses can learn and have short term memory, which is believed to help the
fetus bond with his or her mother. When researchers used vibroacoustic
stimulation, Gonzalez-Gonzalez, Suarez, Perez-Pinero, Armas, Domenech,
and Bartha (2006) asserted that newborns in utero recognized the stimulus.
Newborns habituated sooner than babies who were not stimulated before birth.
This evidence suggests that babies encouraged during fetal life were able to
European Journal of Educational Sciences, Special Edition, October 2019, ISSN: 1857- 6036
80
learn, recall, and memorize. Fetuses demonstrate advancement in cognitive
abilities throughout the fetal development period because the brain is this
amazing organ that has the potential for plasticity, neurogenesis, and the
creation of new neural pathways. Researchers like Otto Rank and Wilfred
Bion hypothesized that being born is a traumatizing experience because the
brain becomes bombarded by outside stimuli, causing the infant to experience
chaos and traumatization.
The hypothesis is not too far-fetched, considering that research
postulated that learning and memory formation occur in utero. Memory and
cognitive abilities learned inside the womb clash with the new environment
when the infant is removed from the womb. The baby takes in an array of
sounds, light, and different touches and smells that were limited in utero. If
the trauma of being born is significant, it is the caregiver’s responsibility to
ensure that a secure attachment is formed, because this lessens the “traumatic
experience” by providing a contextual environment where the infant can
establish trust instead of mistrust. The strength of the mother’s attachment,
mentally and physically, predicates the extensiveness of the trauma or
determines if the traumatic birthing experience will be lessened and managed
by providing love and trust instead of neglect and mistrust. Aside from the
possible functions of recognition and attachment to the mother, the
significance of fetal memory and cognition are necessary for the promotion of
breastfeeding and language acquisition (James, 2010).
Lipina, Colombo, and Jorge (2010) postulated that SES levels are
associated with a degree of hemispheric specialization and gray/white matter
volumes, concluding that reduced language skills were associated with lower
SES and related to less underlying neuronal specialization. According to
James (2010), “A large amount of evidence from habituation, classical
conditioning and exposure learning in humans substantiate that the fetus can
learn; however, there is no sound evidence showing that extra auditory
stimulation is of benefit to child development” (p. 52). Chronic poverty adds
a different variable to how newborns learn; the implications poverty has on
babies as they develop through the various stages of life impact their
education, increase stress levels, exposure to violence, and put them at a higher
risk of being traumatized.
Mounting studies show the detrimental impact of poverty on brain
development in children and adolescents, revealing how and why there is an
achievement gap in education between Blacks, Latinos, and their white
counterpart. Cortisol is the stress hormone needed when there is distress and
the fight and flight response catapults into high gear when the body senses
imminent danger or significant stress. High levels of cortisol affect the
amygdala that regulates emotions, and it can cause coronary heart disease,
hypertension, and high cholesterol levels. Children living in poverty have
European Journal of Educational Sciences, Special Edition, October 2019, ISSN: 1857- 6036
8
1
higher levels of cortisol and other stress markers that have disadvantageous
effects on emotional intelligence, regulation, academics, and executive
functioning. According to Blair and Raver (2016),
Executive function is essential for self-regulation and school readiness
and is a fundamental building block of early cognitive and social competence.
Available evidence indicates that the effects of socioeconomic and early
psychosocial disadvantage on cortisol and brain structure partially mediate
effects of poverty on the development of executive function in childhood.
Impact of poverty on brain development and executive function are likely one
critical pathway, along with reduced stimulation for learning, through which
poverty is associated with gaps in school readiness and achievement and
positive life outcomes. (p. 4)
Taylor and Barrett (2018) professed that “the impact of developmental
trauma in the early years can have a disproportionate effect on the growth,
day-to-day functioning and life chances of a young person” (p.73).
Other studies have shown how nonhumans’ neural pathways evolve,
creating new and advanced channels when placed in environments where they
were stimulated and challenged to interact with devices and manipulatives that
promoted higher levels of cognitive functioning. According to Lipina and
Colombo (2009), elements of executive function, memory, and self-
monitoring tend to be lower in children who live in poverty. The researchers
used basic tasks to “assess skills related to dorsolateral prefrontal (working
memory), anterior cingulate (cognitive control), and ventromedial (reward
processing) prefrontal systems, the results showed a consistent disparity
between children from lower and middle SES groups” (p. 583). As various
studies report, poverty-stricken children are placed at a disadvantage from
birth throughout the different stages of life. If preventive measures,
interventions, and programs are not tailored to meet this demographic, the
cycle of generational poverty will continue, creating a new environmental
DNA passed down to its offspring. How do society and educational systems
close the divide in education, learning, and development? According to Ladd
(2012), “Addressing the educational challenges faced by children from
disadvantaged families will require a broader and bolder approach to
education policy than the recent efforts to reform schools” (p. 204).
Chronic Poverty and Trauma
The majority of psychological research on the consequences of trauma,
poverty, and education has been conducted at the level of the individual,
typically studying the persons’ trauma histories, and trauma exposures with
their symptoms, demographics, and contextual environment (Klest, 2012). As
reported by Collins et al. (2010), there is critical evidence suggesting that
children, adolescents, and families growing up in poverty are more likely to
European Journal of Educational Sciences, Special Edition, October 2019, ISSN: 1857- 6036
82
experience multiple traumas, significant adverse life events, and develop
convoluted symptoms of traumatic distress at disproportionate rates. The
research presented in this literature explores trauma from a specific group that
it impacts and consists of a subset or region of individuals, instead of looking
at trauma as a single catastrophe but a subset of issues or deficiencies that
culminate into traumatic experiences from living a life of generational curses
and chronic poverty. It is no secret that Blacks, Latinos, and other minorities
constitute most of this vulnerable and underserved group. As reported by
Collins et al. (2010), “Efforts to explain the severity and chronicity of reactions
to repeated traumas traditionally focus on the cumulative effects of multiple
traumatic episodes” (p. 11). Trauma can be experienced or witnessed
vicariously, and second-hand trauma can be worse for the person who is
helping the survivor or the one who saw the event from a different perspective.
After the 911 attack in New York, people suffered from posttraumatic stress
as far as Texas and Oklahoma; it is understood as a profoundly disrupting
experience that can threaten the well‐being and safety of those involved
(Gabrielli, Gill, Koester, & Borntrager, 2013).
Many psychologists and researchers describe trauma differently,
characterizing a common thread that ties the theories together, but a definitive
definition of trauma is difficult to establish because tragedy can be subjective.
A clinician and client can disagree about an episode being traumatic or not,
because of the client’s apperception, resiliency, and personal growth. Scott and
Briere (2015) discussed the ambiguous description of trauma that is
characterized by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Disorders, 5th
edition (DSM-5), which provides a broad definition with limitations, causing
conflicting information in determining if an event satisfies statistical
definitions of trauma. Scott and Briere (2015) denoted their interpretation and
professed that “an event is traumatic if it is extremely upsetting, at least
temporarily overwhelms the individual’s internal resources, and produces
lasting psychological symptoms” (p. 10). If the definition defined by the DSM-
5 is adhered to strictly, many clients would not be classified as a trauma
survivor.
There is a fallacy that a traumatic event must be a devastating isolated
experience that keeps resurfacing in a person’s life. Trauma does not have to
be a single catastrophe but a culmination of small, insignificant issues that
converge together until all the pieces of life’s problems connect like the perfect
jigsaw puzzle. The mental symptomology of traumatic experiences can
debilitate people from being able to access coping skills to keep intrusive
thoughts at bay, and they lack the cognitive abilities to manage and process
the traumatic experience and the possible manifestation of psychological
disorders. It takes a thorough understanding of the ramifications of trauma and
the unfolding developmental processes of childhood as it is experienced in a
European Journal of Educational Sciences, Special Edition, October 2019, ISSN: 1857- 6036
83
particular culture to understand the nature of historical trauma, as aggregated
spiritual, physical, emotional, and psychological wounding over the lifespan
and across generations (Gabrielli, Gill, Koester, & Borntrager, 2013). Trauma
that affects poverty-stricken cultures covers a broad range of atrocities that
destroys the sense of safety within the individuals’ community, including
neighborhoods, schools, churches, towns, and other places where people
conjugate together but separately (“Center for Substance Abuse,” 2014).
Studies have identified the problem and determined that increased
trauma is associated with poverty and deprivation. The consistent restriction
and limited opportunities that characterize poverty can lead to daily hassles
and unexpected traumatic events like the lack of food or disconnection from
water, electricity, and other supplies (Shamai, 2017). It is normal to experience
trauma across the lifespan; for most people, individuals and communities
usually respond to tragedy with resilience, because many influences shape the
effects of trauma among individuals and families. Due to other factors and
circumstances, it is not just the traumatic episode that predicts the outcome,
but also the episode’s context and the interactions between family support, first
responders, counselors, psychological first aid providers, and community
leaders (“Center for Substance Abuse,” 2014). Severe childhood adversity, in
the form of traumas such as physical and sexual abuse, alters young people’s
transition into adulthood due to social and behavioral reasons, but also due to
the physiologic and neurobiological changes that occur due to chronic
stressors. Considering the impact and effects of poverty and trauma, Myers et
al. (2015) proclaimed that numerous studies refute the results expected
because both African Americans and Latinos(as) appeared to be rather
resilient, despite their higher than normal stressors and burdens of adversities
and traumatizing experiences. It is safe to infer that people who have lived a
life of trauma and lack resources all of their lives have become conditioned to
become content in whatever state they find themselves in their contextual
world. They do not know the hidden rules of the middle-class, so what appears
to be poverty and trauma for the middle-class is healthy everyday living for
people residing in poverty.
Research substantiates that fifty percent of people will experience
trauma in their lifetime. This traumatic experience for only fifty percent of the
population is up for debate if we believe in the work of Otto Rank who
proclaimed that being born is a traumatic experience, beginning when the
infant inhaled his or her first breath. Stewart (2014) purported that Arthur
Schopenhauer and Otto Rank discussed suffering because of birth itself,
referring to being born as a traumatic experience. Aside from birth, people in
poverty are exposed to the daily hassles that cause stress. Klest (2012) reported
that models of trauma and coping include the erosion of family processes and
dynamics (structure, relations, coping) in the context of community violence
European Journal of Educational Sciences, Special Edition, October 2019, ISSN: 1857- 6036
84
and poverty, explaining the higher likelihood of family violence in the
contextual environment of poverty. The traumatic experience by one family
member can create a traumatic stress reaction in other family members through
association and proximity to the danger or the catastrophe (Kiser & Black,
2005).
The Domino Effect: Chronic Poverty, Trauma, and Bullying
Today, school violence is nothing new, and it continues to become
more frequent and deadly in the school systems. Across the globe, educational
institutions have active-shooter drills protocols and prepare students, faculty,
administrators, and staff what to do when this vicious act of violence occurs.
No matter the amount of preparation, no one is ever prepared for the time a
school shooting happens in their contextual framework. This type of school
violence is evident, reported by newspapers, talk shows, television, social
media, and other mediums, repeatedly. As published by Levers (2012), it took
the U.S. Department of Education and the U.S. Secret Service to define the
parameters of school violence, but one of the most critical findings was
“defining school violence as consisting of two sorts: lethal and nonlethal.”
This research focuses on the nonlethal violence that does not get the attention
warranted, and protocols are not protecting the children and ensuring their
safety sufficiently. Nonlethal school violence consists of all forms of bullying
threats, intimidation, harassment, assault, and sexual assault, constituting
physical, verbal, social, and cyberbullying (Levers, 2012). According to
Skaine (2015), “bullying is an unwanted, aggressive behavior that involves a
real or perceived power imbalance that is repeated, or has the potential to be
repeated, over time” (p. 40). Bullied children and adolescents have PTSD, and
they need to be treated for the symptoms because traumatized children grow
up to become traumatized, broken adults. Herman (2001) reported the
significance of abuse in childhood, and how later in life, childhood abuse can
contribute to increased risk for cancer, heart disease, stroke, and obesity –
increasing the risk of alcoholism, depression, suicide attempts, self-harm, and
absenteeism from work in adulthood. Bullying is a form of complex
posttraumatic stress disorder (CPTSD). Herman (2012) reported that “CPTSD
was specifically associated with early-onset and long duration of trauma; it
was very rarely found in survivors of natural disasters, and very commonly
found in survivors of childhood abuse” (p. 25).
The reoccurrence of the same traumatic experience is what
distinguishes CPTSD from PTSD. An individual might have PTSD after a
horrific car accident, an isolated sexual assault, or natural disaster, but CPTSD
is characterized as having the same traumatic experience repeated on several
occasions. Research substantiates that CPTSD symptoms account for the
functional impairment of trauma survivors who endured prolonged and
European Journal of Educational Sciences, Special Edition, October 2019, ISSN: 1857- 6036
85
repeated tragic episodes exceeding the damage attributed to PTSD alone
(Herman, 2012). Bullying falls under the category of complex trauma, and it
attributes to the psychological effects in adulthood. Children living in poverty
can stand out from the crowd because of dirty clothes, holes in shoes, unkempt
hair, and poor personal hygiene. Bilic (2015) reported that almost all
socioeconomic variables that suggest that a family is poor correlate
significantly with victimization, and the results from the same study denoted
that 34.8% of those living in poverty responded violently toward peers because
of their more inferior financial status, and 45.7% were bullied and victimized
for being poor. Some bullied children might retaliate by taking extreme violent
means of retaliation. In the 1990s, 12 of the 15 school shooting cases that
occurred, the shooters had a history of being bullied (Skaine, 2015).
Trauma is an occurrence when an intense experience stuns a child like
a bolt out of the blue, overwhelming the child, leaving him or her altered and
disconnected from body, mind, and spirit (Steele & Malchiodi, 2012).
Bullying is not harmless; bullying is not child’s play; bullying is not a rite of
passage. A rite of passage insinuates that bullying is part of life and the school
experience, so children should expect to be bullied. Children go to school to
learn about history, science, arithmetic, spelling, and using correct grammar,
so being bullied is one of the soft skills or indirect learning experiences
embedded in the school curriculum. In the academic subjects, students receive
a letter grade of pass or fail as they matriculate through school. What grade do
students get for enduring four, six, eight, or twelve years of bullying? How do
teachers indicate if their students passed or failed? If the student never
attempts suicide, is that a passing score?
If the student only has low self-esteem and depression and anxiety, is
that considered acceptable to be promoted to another year of punishment? If
the individual only has non-suicidal injuries – such as cutting or burning – is
that enough for promotion? However, when the person dies by suicide, it is
safe to say that he or she failed the rite of passage. Many children believe that
when they get older, they will grow up and out of the bullying environment,
but the psychological effects and ramifications follow them into adulthood.
Tariq (2011) purported that the problem of most significant concern is
the stifling effects produced by the survivor of bullying. Staggering numbers
of investigators have asked whether victims of peer aggression experience
psychosocial maladjustments, such as depression, anxiety, and low self-
esteem, and “it is clearly important to know the answer to this question, so that
children’s distress does not go unrecognized” (p. 23). Suicide rates have
increased tremendously over time in schools, contributing to the fact that those
who are being bullied and lack evidence of physical scars, but they suffer away
in silence until victimization becomes overbearing, losing hope, and deciding
that the only solution for them is to die by suicide (Hendricks & Tanga, 2019).
European Journal of Educational Sciences, Special Edition, October 2019, ISSN: 1857- 6036
86
Bullying is not a rite of passage – even though children are steadfast in
believing that their trials and tribulations will be over when they become
adults. Unfortunately, the research does not predict a happily ever after but a
possible lifetime of physical and psychological problems – leaving mental,
spiritual, emotional, and physical wounds. Parents and educators must
recognize the signs and symptoms of bullying. Often, the physical signs are
evident – like unexplained injuries, bruises, broken bones, and the black eye.
Bullied children tend to isolate themselves from family and friends, become
depressed, and become overly anxious. Vanderbilt and Augustyn (2010)
identified several red flags that an individual is being bullied. Some of the
more common indications are stomachaches, headaches, insomnia, social
problems, lack of friends, and academic failure. It is critical that bullying is
considered on an extensive list of differential diagnoses because bullying may
overlap with other conditions such as medical illness, learning problems, and
psychological disorders (Vanderbilt & Augustyn, 2010).
Adults who were bullied as a child or adolescent may have a lower
socioeconomic status (poverty), engage in risky behaviors, suffer from anxiety
and depression, and experience psychosis and have suicidal ideations or
attempts. Blad (2016) stated that children should be assessed from birth to
understand the extent of bullying on psychosis later in their adult life – with
some groups to be nearly five times more likely to suffer from episodes of
psychosis by the age of 18. In adulthood, bullied children have higher rates of
suicidal thoughts and plans than children who had not been bullied (Wagner,
2016). There is no escape when bullied children grow up to be high school
dropouts and experience long-term effects (depression, relationship problems,
psychosis, substance use disorder, risky behaviors, and suicide). When the
psychological effects of bullying accompany children into adolescent and
adulthood, not only did the children fail the rite of passage, but parents failed;
educators failed; church leaders failed; the school system failed; society failed
these children.
Why is it critical that bullying is discussed trauma and the chronicity
of poverty? It is the psychological effects of bullying that warrants the nation’s
attention if the educational gap is going to be closed. Being bullied as a child
or experiencing bullying behaviors have the propensity to continue the cycle
of chronic poverty, so the generational curse continues with no end if a cultural
shift does not happen soon.
• Children who live in poverty experience higher rates of trauma and
bullying.
• Children who are bullied are more likely to drop out of school,
continuing to expand the educational divide and the continuation of chronic
poverty.
European Journal of Educational Sciences, Special Edition, October 2019, ISSN: 1857- 6036
87
• They have a lower social, economic status, suffer from mental
disorders, engage in risky behaviors, experience psychosis, and have a higher
risk of having a substance use disorder.
• Bullied children have a difficult time keeping a job at the age of 24 to
26, and men who were bullied in high school had a significant chance of being
unemployed at the age of 50 (Brimblecombe, Evans-Lacko, Knapp, King,
Takizawa, Maughan, & Arsenault, 2008).
Matthews, Jennings, Lee, & Pardini (2017) discussed research
showing that bullied children had a low SES, and they are less likely to have
a high school diploma or GED. Throughout their lifetime, they have lower
adult family income and lack adequate resources with lower social status in
their community. Brimblecombe et al. (2008) reported that “four decades after
the bullying occurred, both men and women who were bullied in childhood
were less likely to be in employment and accumulated less wealth in the form
of home-ownership or savings than participants who were not bullied” (p.
138). Research depicting those results can only strengthen chronic poverty
with a new generation of people who will continue to reinforce and ensure
generational poverty advances for the foreseeable future.
TF-CBT
The best method for treating bullying is prevention and having a zero-
tolerance in school and at home. McAdams and Schmidt (2007) purported that
teachers have indicated feeling underprepared to recognize and manage bullies
and bullying behaviors in their classroom. Their lack of knowledge causes
them to ignore serious aggressive behaviors, or they identify bullying but feel
inadequate or too afraid to intervene and address the problem. Parents and
guardians are not equipped to train their children and convey the dangers of
bullying, model a bully-proof home, and they lack the knowledge of teaching
their children how to become resilient. Bullying that happens in the home is
often overlooked by everyone involved because it is family-oriented. Bowes,
Wolke, Joinson, Lereya, and Lewis (2014) proclaimed that sibling bullying is
a specific type of aggressive behavior that is repeated over time (complex
trauma) – intending to cause harm and dominate the weaker sibling. Studies
show a strong correlation between sibling bullying and increased internalizing
symptoms – resulting in the emergence of depression and self-harm in
children, adolescent, and adulthood. It is essential to protect children in the
two most important places where they spend a significant amount of time–
home and school.
Children must become emotionally intelligent, develop coping skills,
and use assertive communication (verbal and non-verbal) to deescalate and
disengage from bullying behaviors and bullies. If a child is not safe within
one’s mind, he is not safe anywhere. The renewing of their mind must
European Journal of Educational Sciences, Special Edition, October 2019, ISSN: 1857- 6036
88
transform children by becoming bully-proof. Bullying survivors may have
PTSD, and they do whatever it takes to escape the intrusive thoughts, shame,
and humiliation of being demoralized. Steele and Malchiodi (2012) professed
that “PTSD is more common in children than most physicians believe;
therefore, the diagnosis can often be missed. Children being treated for
behavioral problems may be suffering from PTSD” (p. 15). Black,
Woodworth, Tremblay, and Carpenter (2012) proclaimed that “TF-CBT is a
component-based model and can be summarized using the acronym
“PRACTICE” (p. 196). Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) is an empirically
supported approach to treating PTSD and CPTSD. According to Levers
(2012), through the method, clients learn how to reduce negative emotional
and behavioral responses that follow a traumatic event, because “the treatment
is based, fundamentally, on learning and cognitive theories that address
distorted beliefs and attributions related to the traumatic events experienced
by an individual” (p. 496). As reported by Black et al. (2012),
In relation to treating trauma-related symptoms, there are a number of
CBT-like treatments that involve most aspects of the therapy but do not
actually present themselves specifically as CBT, such as multimodal trauma
treatment (MMTT), as well as different variations of CBT, such as trauma-
focused cognitive behavioural therapy (TF-CBT). (p. 194)
TF-CBT is the treatment modality discussed in this paper as an
efficient approach to treating bullying survivors suffering from symptoms of
CPTSD. Johnson (2012) reported that trauma-focused care is done
professionally and thoughtfully. It is informed of the overall construct
showing great concern not only for the survivor’s emotional safety but it is
focused on the reassessment and restructuring of the treatment system to
evaluate how the traumatic experience may affect the survivor’s role and
interaction with certain aspects of the entire systematic approach for treatment.
One of the many benefits of TF-CBT is that multimodalities can be applied to
reach the desired goals of hope, recovery, and resiliency, and the approach is
substantiated by years of empirical and evidence-based research and
advancing theories to wellness. As explained by Black et al. (2012),
During treatment, the traumatized child is provided with
psychoeducation, taught relaxation skills, affective expression and
modulation, and cognitive coping skills. In addition, they are encouraged to
use trauma narration and cognitively process the trauma, use in vivo exposure
to master trauma reminders, have conjoint parent-child sessions, and enhance
safety and the trajectory of development. (p. 196)
Johnson (2012) stated that TF-CBT is the most studied of all the
treatments for trauma-related symptoms among children and adolescents, even
though many professionals do not recognize the advantages. Black et al.
(2012) asserted that regardless of the specific treatment protocol that is
European Journal of Educational Sciences, Special Edition, October 2019, ISSN: 1857- 6036
89
adhered to, it is crucial to keep in mind that psychoeducation, coping skills,
trauma narratives, cognitive restructuring, and creating an action plan for after
the cessation of therapy are all typically integral practices of the trauma-
informed treatment programs. By implementing some or all of these critical
practices, it should be possible to reduce trauma-related symptoms among
trauma survivors. Even resilient children show distress following trauma
exposure, and their reactions cause them to increase monitoring of their
environment for potential dangers, experiencing anxiety when they are
separated from trusted adults, or heightened need for affection, support, and
reassurance because trauma re-enactments may resurface again in their lives
(Kiser & Black, 2005). Therefore, treating the psychological effects of
bullying is critical to living a life free of chronic poverty. It is estimated that
160,000 school-aged children miss school because of bullies or bullying
behaviors (Skaine, 2017). If children are too afraid or too humiliated to go to
school, how will they ever learn the hidden rules of the middle-class and
become educated, productive members of society?
Chronic Poverty and the Educational Divide
Individuals who live in poverty present a profound challenge in school
systems, homes, and communities. Brito and Noble (2014) asserted that
socioeconomically disadvantaged children tend to experience less linguistic,
social, and cognitive stimulation from their caregivers and home environments
than children from higher Socioeconomic Status (SES) homes” (p. 2). Hair,
Hanson, Wolfe, and Pollak (2015) proclaimed that there is strong evidence
that poverty influences language (temporal lobe) and executive functioning
(frontal lobe). Deficits in the executive functioning of individuals in poverty
have been found during the life course in studies conducted during infancy as
well as in childhood, adolescence, and adulthood (p. 823). Mending the issues
associated with chronic poverty and breaking generational curses is a massive
undertaking, but several educational programs are intervening at an early age
in the child’s psychological and psychosocial development, but those
initiatives alone do not alleviate the problems or lessen the psychological
effects of poverty and trauma. Hair, Hanson, Wolfe, and Pollak (2015)
reported that “children living in poverty have lower scores on standardized
tests of academic achievement, more mediocre grades in school, and lower
educational attainment (p. 823).
Parents must be taught and trained to become self-sufficient, improve
self-esteem and self-compassion and learn to have high self-efficacy and the
will and desire to change. Before tackling the trauma that is interwoven in
poverty that causes a reoccurring cycle, society has to take an aggressive
approach to improve the physical conditions, educate and train parents, and
combat hunger and other lack of resources. Ruby Payne wrote a book entitle,
European Journal of Educational Sciences, Special Edition, October 2019, ISSN: 1857- 6036
90
A Framework for Understanding Poverty. According to Payne (1996), if
people around a person has similar circumstances, and the problems attached
to poverty, the notion of poverty and wealth is vague. Individuals bring the
hidden rules of their environment, placing poverty-stricken communities at a
disadvantage because schools and society operate from the middle-class
norms and use the arcane rules of the middle class. The two most important
conditions to help people move out of poverty are education and relationships
(Payne, 1996).
How is it possible for parents to teach their children the hidden rules
of common cultural patterns when all they know is the exposed rules of
poverty? There must be a cultural shift to train parents to become
knowledgeable and educated because education is the vehicle that will drive
them away from poverty. The effects of generational poverty cannot be
obliterated in an instance. It takes significant time to unravel generations of
neural pathways that have been programmed to accept poverty. Society and
educators must be willing to be patient, teach, and reprogram a culture of
people by transforming their mental faculties. Amatea and West (2007) stated
that rather than viewing poverty from society’s perspective, inclusive of
systemic influences and class privilege, many of these educators believe that
poor people are intrinsically inferior because of their innate flaws such as lack
of motivation or poor decision making. Parents and children living in poverty
cannot be held accountable for their intrinsic defects. Until the generational
narrative of poverty has been retold psychologically, spiritually, and
emotionally, the problem will remain in today’s societies. Poor people will
always exist in the world; however, educators and society must be willing to
lead, follow, or get out of the way, because if they are not ready to be part of
the solution, their patronizing demeanor is part of the problem.
The educational gap between Blacks and Latinos and Whites has
nothing to do with the intellectual abilities of the poverty-stricken minority
population. In many minority families, getting a good education is not
emphasized because survival is more important than learning. Children living
in poverty do not receive support or challenges that are necessary to be
successful in high school, college, and life. Since education is not essential to
their parents, why should they make it vital for themselves? Some minority
students overcome the odds and become successful; however, for the majority,
these neglected students fall between the cracks.
There is a need to train parents on how to educate and instill the value
of education in their children. van der Veen and Preece (2005) asserted that
“the trend in adult education policies (where they exist at all) has been to focus
on literacy or basic education. Research has shown that basic literacy skills
are not in themselves sufficient to make a significant impact on poverty
reduction, though they do help” (p. 381). Therefore, it would be beneficial to
European Journal of Educational Sciences, Special Edition, October 2019, ISSN: 1857- 6036
91
provide workshops, seminars, and hands-on training for parents of minorities
living in poverty, but more is required in areas where a tremendous impact can
be made to improve living conditions. van der Veen and Preece (2005) stated
that targeting the real needs of the poor will take a collaborative approach
between all stakeholders, employers, businesses, and employees. Poverty is an
eyesore, and it does not benefit society, families, education, and the economy.
Many parents do an excellent job rearing respectful citizens; however, they
fail to train them to be self-sufficient, self-motivated, and educationally
prepared to attend school, college, and productive members of society. When
children are prepared at home, they come to school at an advantage, and they
are eager to learn. A healthy family with solid values is the foundation for
academic and personal success. Through adult education, what better way to
model the importance of learning and to work to improve living conditions.
The evidence on adult education in the public economic domain leads to the
hypothesis that a “more extended and more targeted system for basic
education, agricultural extension, and vocational training is urgently needed
to help people to generate income” (van der Veen & Preece, 2005, 390). If the
root (family) is not nurtured and watered, the possibility for success and
motivation will wither and die.
Teacher’s Perception and Stereotype Threats
According to Dye (2014), research from previous studies revealed that
teachers often doubt that education is vital for low-income students of color,
and they have feelings of inadequacy when teaching minorities who live in
poverty. The confident educator influences students’ lives in ways
unimaginable until many years later, when the former students share their
experiences about that one instructor who made a difference. Many influential
people contribute their success to a memorable teacher who encouraged them
to be the best they could be in the classroom and beyond. Research has
provided enough evidence to support that minorities do not learn the same way
as their counterpart, and the same criterion does not motivate them. Educators
must create culturally diverse environments, have belief in their pedagogical
skills and abilities to connect and teach all students. Studies reported that
educators could feel unequipped to teach culturally diverse students, reporting
lower self-efficacy. Tucker, Porter, Reinke, Herman, Ivery, Mack, and
Jackson (2005) postulated that a lack of efficacy could explain the educational
divide between minorities living in poverty and the middle-class; however,
school administrators must increase efforts to improve teacher efficacy
because it is paramount to decrease the disproportionate high school dropout
rates and underperforming children in the classroom.
When children are neglected, abused, and experience trauma, teachers
are usually on the frontline, being a friend, mother-figure, and voice of reason.
European Journal of Educational Sciences, Special Edition, October 2019, ISSN: 1857- 6036
92
Teachers need to know their students’ culture. According to Aldermann
(2004), cultural competence helps educators to become astute to verbal and
nonverbal cues from minorities, creating a classroom climate that encourages
students to express themselves and feel accepted without losing their identity.
School personnel, administrators, staff, and educators must see that there is a
need to improvise and devise a plan to address the educational needs of
culturally diverse students living in poverty. Cultural sensitivity is a process
that will prevent stereotype threats in the classroom that decrease bias,
prejudice, and demoralization that stifles the learning process.
Neuburger, Jansen, Heil, and Quaiser-Pohl (2012) purported that
“stereotype threat is the event of a negative stereotype about a group to which
one belongs becoming self-relevant and to being at risk of confirming, as self-
characteristic, a negative stereotype about one’s group” (p. 62). Stereotype
threat can disrupt educational performance, causing low self-efficacy, low
self-esteem, and a significant decrease in academic motivation. Studies show
that the threat of stereotype causes minorities to not only struggle
academically, but it asphyxiates personal growth and social development.
Stereotype threat is indoctrinated in the belief that anyone in a stereotypical
group can feel pressure to avoid fulfilling negative expectations. The pressure
of becoming a self-fulfilling prophecy bolsters underperformance, which is
the most detrimental barrier to achievement. There is no doubt that minorities
have equal potential to perform as well as their counterpart; however, when
minority students know that they are being compared, they tend to perform
discouragingly.
As reported by Neuburger et al. (2012), stereotype threat debilitates
performance because it initiates three different mechanisms that reduce the
capacity of working memory (physiological stress, monitoring processes, and
emotion-suppressing efforts). The mechanisms evoke a cognitive imbalance
between the person’s concept of self, ability domain, and group. Stereotype
threat accentuates minorities perception of being inferior; therefore, they take
on an inferiority complex of their own, contributing to their lack of confidence
in their abilities, accepting the façade that white students are smarter than they
and educators view them as inept. The research does not support that
stereotype threats occur in white teachers only but any educator who feels a
particular population, such as those living in poverty, is beneath them and they
hold poverty-stricken people responsible for a life of deprivation. Amatea and
West (2007) proclaimed that Many educators view poor people as morally and
culturally deficient. Believing that poor families have attitudes, values, and
behaviors that sustain their position at the bottom of the economic ladder, these
educators often blame parents for passing on these traits to their children
instead of transmitting the middle-class cultural patterns they believe are
necessary to succeed in school and in life. (p. 83)
European Journal of Educational Sciences, Special Edition, October 2019, ISSN: 1857- 6036
93
Research supports that students who can identify with their academics
and self-esteem have a better chance of being successful. Minority students
try to accomplish goals and achieve a sound education as everyone else, but
chronic poverty and teacher’s perception cause them to struggle and fall
behind. To succeed, minorities living in poverty are trying too hard because
they are dealing with the threat of stereotype and disidentification. It could be
assumed that hard work, dedication, and commitment to educational
endeavors would close the gap and help minorities excel; however, trying
harder adds an extra burden, causing more problems such as higher levels of
stress, anxiety, and depression. What options are left for minority children
living in poverty?
Implications and Conclusion
Living a life of chronic poverty that increases the risk of trauma,
victimization, bullying and its psychological effects, becoming a high school
dropout, and being educated by a system that was not designed for people
living in deprivation is not a situation that anyone would choose. Trauma plays
a significant role in the lives of people living in poverty. Traumatic
experiences are subjective, but the concept of trauma suggests that the
wounded is confronted by an enormous life-changing event, such as divorce,
death of a loved one, unemployment or terminated, diagnosis of illness or
disease, or culture shock; however, trauma is generally referred to a severe or
psychological injury sustained as a result of a life-threatening or horrific
experience (Stebnicki, 2017). Bullying is a significant problem for children,
and the epidemic warrants the attention of policymakers, educators, parents,
church leaders, and community stakeholders. Society is doing a poor job of
protecting and educating children. Vanderbilt and Augustyn (2010) discussed
the significance of people being more vigilant and aggressive by not tolerating
bullying and modeling that it is not welcomed in the home, community, and
school. Particular focus must be placed on children or adolescents who have
chronic medical illnesses like acne, being medically obsessed, severe eczema,
physical deformities like cleft lip and palate, or some who suffer from
neuropsychological disorders like learning disabilities, down syndrome, and
autism spectrum disorder, making them potential targets for bullying and
demoralization. School bullying is a widespread phenomenon not only in the
United States of America but in many countries around the world, signifying
that bullying should be considered a significant international public health
problem (Collier, 2013).
Research proclaims that today’s school counselors can bring unique
skills to help educators working with students in high-poverty schools who
experience bullying and the psychological effects. Working with this
population provides a significant opportunity for school counselors to broaden
European Journal of Educational Sciences, Special Edition, October 2019, ISSN: 1857- 6036
94
their role with educators, staff, office personnel, and administrators beyond
only delivering services to individual students but taking a collaborative
approach (Amatea & West, 2007). Poverty-stricken minorities can accomplish
great things academically when teachers and others in the learning
environment create an atmosphere that makes them feel confident. Children
can learn and achieve at very high levels, once they gain confidence in their
abilities and accept that their thinking and efforts can help them do well. It
takes a cumulative effort to make a substantial change in the classroom, and
everyone must accept his or her role. Amatea and West (2007) declared that
These roles are (a) serving as a cultural bridge between teachers and
students and blocking the blaming that often derails efforts to work with poor
students and their families, (b) functioning as a pedagogical partner with
teachers by connecting the curriculum more directly to students’ lives, and (c)
teaming with teachers to create a more welcoming, family-centric school
climate. (p. 82)
School systems and administrators must make the cultural
transformation a priority and not an afterthought or placed at the bottom of the
school’s agenda. Neuburger et al. (2012) declared that stereotype threat and
self-fulfilling prophecies play an instrumental role in socialization and are part
of the hidden curriculum in schools. Adding the hidden curriculum to the
hidden rules of the middle-class, minorities living in poverty do not have a
fighting chance and will remain hidden in the classroom to continue the cycle
of generational poverty. Head Start, 2 Gen Programs, Babies “Can’t” Wait,
and other programs have been established to address the disparity in the
vulnerable population; however, a tremendous amount of work must be put
into action along with robust educational reforms.
No Child Left Behind (NCLB) was designed to ensure that all schools
meet the same academic standards and achievement levels, regardless of the
school system was in an area where poverty is high or not. Research proves
that the NCLB has been nothing but an ultimate failure because state and
federal standards cannot establish guidelines on the hidden rules of education
that was designed for the middle-class. Ladd (2012) reported that under
NCLB, “each school must meet the same standard, regardless of whether it
serves low‐ or high‐SES students and must do so for all relevant subgroups
within the school defined by income, minority status, and Limited English
Proficient status” (p. 213). Poverty-stricken children will always be a general
population of people in the middle-class classroom, skewing achievement
expectations and standards. What must happen for educators to improve their
low self-efficacy and lack of confidence to teach children from lower SES?
Teachers must become culturally competent, demonstrating relevant
pedagogy techniques that encourage educators to understand local students,
cultures, demographics, and the hidden rules of poverty. It is equally crucial
European Journal of Educational Sciences, Special Edition, October 2019, ISSN: 1857- 6036
95
for middle-class teachers to understand children and parents living in
deprivation as it is for people living in poverty to learn the hidden rules of the
middle-class. Matsko and Hammerness (2014) suggested that multicultural
education create dialogue and relevancy to bridge the gap between educators,
students, state and federal standards, and the school’s curriculum. The faculty
must understand the significance of the teacher-student relationship, their
respective cultures, and how the dynamics of education are interwoven in the
classroom. Teachers do not have to go to an elite college or university to reach,
teach, and empower children living in poverty. They must be willing to accept
the fact that these children will always be their classrooms, become flexible
and willing to change to become the best educator for all students and take
those teachable moments to learn more about those students who come from
a different geographical area. Mattsko and Hammerness (2014) cited that
educators must demonstrate how “context extends well beyond one’s
immediate physical surroundings, and in doing so, we illustrate how a simple
understanding of context can be expanded to include state and federal policy,
the neighborhood, the district, and urban public school classroom” (p. 137).
This burden of educating the underserved does not fall solely on the
educational system, but the parents must take a proactive approach to improve
their education and the education of their children. Education starts at home
with family and community. Families must provide stability, the foundation
for education, and relay the importance of learning to their children. When a
family gives their children support, challenges, and stimulate their children,
minorities are more likely to choose harder subjects in high school, get better
grades, end up in better colleges, and have higher self-esteem in college and
beyond. Challenges empower minority children by giving them a vision,
direction, focus, and perseverance to succeed. A new approach called 2 Gen
helps families break the cycle of poverty by simultaneously addressing the
needs of parents and children to improve significant outcomes for the entire
family (“Two-Generation Approaches,” 2018). Familial support provides the
solace that allows freedom and closes the door on stress, worries, and fears.
Parents living in poverty must become educated and learn to be their children’s
first teacher. According to “Two-Generation Approaches” (2018), “Two-
generation strategies can be used in many policy areas, including human
services, education, labor and workforce, and health. They can be used to
address a myriad of policy issues, including poverty, literacy, school
readiness, and family economic stability” (p. 3).
The research presented here does not touch the surface of what it will
take to disassociate poverty from bullying, the disproportionate rate of
traumatic experiences, and the complicated process of closing the educational
divide by educating all students regardless of low socioeconomic status,
demographics, cultures, races, and ethnicities of the underserved, vulnerable
European Journal of Educational Sciences, Special Edition, October 2019, ISSN: 1857- 6036
96
population. Payne (1996) purported that “support systems need to include the
teaching of procedural self-talk, positive self-talk, planning, goal-setting,
coping strategies, appropriate relationships, options during problem-solving,
access to information and know-how, and connections to additional resources”
(p. 75). To make changes and see significant differences, society must be
willing to challenge the status quo and change the culture by breaking the cycle
of generational poverty – equipping people living below the poverty level to
use education as the catalyst to move from poverty to middle-class – where the
hidden rules to function in society are available for everyone.
The United States is an individualistic society, and too many people
are only concerned about their small microcosm. It is going to take a
collectivist approach to stomp out the harmful effects of bullying and its
detrimental ramifications later in life. Bullying is not a rite of passage, and if
not treated, they will carry the trauma and hidden scars into their adult life.
Society must stop considering bullying as a school ritual but recognize it as a
public health crisis. Skaine (2015) reported that 19 percent of U.S. elementary
students are bullied, and more than 160,000 kids stay home from school
because they are afraid as published by Vanderbilt and Augustyn (2010),
bullying, whether as bullies, victims and bully-victims, is correlated with
poorer outcomes in the classroom and society. Bullying involvement is
significant because it can lead to poor psychosocial adjustment, greater health
problems, poverty, and deficient emotional and social adjustment (p. 316).
Bullying behaviors and their lasting effects have the propensity to continue
the cycle of chronic poverty, so the generational curse continues with no end
if a cultural shift does not happen soon. People who live in poverty experience
higher rates of trauma and bullying. Children who are bullied are more likely
to drop out of school, have a lower social, economic status, suffer from mental
disorders, engage in risky behaviors, and substance use disorders. According
to Matthews, Jennings, Lee, & Pardini (2017), research showed that bullied
children had a low SES, and they are less likely to have a high school diploma
or GED. Throughout their lifetime, they have lower adult family income and
lack adequate resources with lower social status in their community. Studies
depicting those results strengthen chronic poverty with a new generation of
people contributing to the poverty-stricken life cycle.
Taylor and Barrett (2018) professed that raising awareness of early
trauma is a critical first step to reduce its impact, but most of the contemporary
literature focuses on the actual incidence of trauma. As denoted, trauma is not
necessarily an isolated incident but the culmination of traumatic events and
episodes in the contextual environment. Society must construct a plan that
addresses the mental, physical, psychological, and cognitive faculties of all
individuals living in poverty. 2 Gen is very promising because it focuses on
educating the parents, children, and community by working from the inside
European Journal of Educational Sciences, Special Edition, October 2019, ISSN: 1857- 6036
97
out. According to Taylor and Barrett (2018), educators cannot prevent
traumatic experiences from occurring; however, they can seek to mitigate their
impact by implementing a range of approaches and interventions such as
R4L,” Readiness for Learning (R4L), a ‘Brain-based, Attachment-Led,
Trauma-Informed Community intervention’ (BALTIC) approach to
supporting learners to be settled and ready to learn” (p. 65). Collected data
indicates that R4L has made significant progress in the classroom with a
proven increase in students concerning self-regulation and executive
functioning (Taylor & Barret, 2018). In summation, Ladd (2012) reported that
policymakers must assure that children attending schools that educate large
proportions of disadvantaged children must provide them access to high-
quality teachers, principals, supports for students, and other resources, holding
school systems accountable for the quality of education provided and their
internal processes and practices.
References:
1. Alderman, M. K. (2004). Motivation for achievement: Possibilities for
teaching and learning (2nd ed). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
2. Amatea, E. S., & West-Olatunji, C. (2007). Joining the conversation
about educating our poorest children: Emerging leadership roles for
school counselors in high-poverty schools. Professional School
Counseling, 11(2), 81-89.
3. Bilic, V. (2015). The role of socioeconomic differences and material
deprivation in peer violence. Epiphany, 8(2), 177-202.
doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.21533/epiphany.v8i2.169.g137
4. Black, P. J., Woodworth, M., Tremblay, M., & Carpenter, T. (2012).
A review of trauma-informed treatment for adolescents. Canadian
Psychology, 53(3), 192-203.
5. Blad, E. (2016). Bullying: Preventing bullying through science, policy,
and practice. Education Week, 35(31)
6. Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2016). Poverty, stress, and brain
development: New directions for prevention and intervention.
Academic pediatrics, 16(3 Suppl), S30–S36.
doi:10.1016/j.acap.2016.01.010
7. Bowes, L., Wolke, D., Joinson, C., Lereya, S. T., & Lewis, G. (2014).
Sibling bullying and risk of depression, anxiety, and self-harm: A
prospective cohort study. Pediatrics, 134(4). doi:10.1542/peds.2014-
0832
8. Brimblecombe, N., Evans-Lacko, S., Knapp, M., King, D., Takizawa,
R., Maughan, B., & Arseneault, L. (2018). Long term economic impact
associated with childhood bullying victimisation. Social Science and
European Journal of Educational Sciences, Special Edition, October 2019, ISSN: 1857- 6036
98
Medicine, 208, 134-141.
doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.05.014
9. Brito, N. H., & Noble, K. G. (2014). Socioeconomic status and
structural brain development. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 8, 276.
doi:10.3389/fnins.2014.00276
10. Briere, J. N., & Scott, C. (2015). Principles of trauma therapy: A guide
to symptoms, evaluation, and treatment (2nd ed., p. 10). Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
11. Center for Substance Abuse Treatment (US). Trauma-informed care in
behavioral health services. Rockville (MD): Substance Abuse and
Mental Health Services Administration (US); 2014. (Treatment
Improvement Protocol (TIP) Series, No. 57.) Chapter 2, Trauma
Awareness. Available from:
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK207203/
12. Collier, R. (2013). Bullying symptoms. Canadian Medical Association
Journal, 85(16), E754. doi.10.1503/cmaj.109-4610
13. Collins, K., Connors, K., Donohue, A., Gardner, S., Goldblatt, E.,
Hayward, A., Kiser, L., Strieder, F. Thompson, E. (2010).
Understanding the impact of trauma and urban poverty on family
systems: Risks, resilience, and interventions. Baltimore, MD: Family
Informed Trauma Treatment.
http://nctsn.org/nccts/nav.do?pid=ctr_rsch_prod_ar or
http://fittcenter.umaryland.edu/WhitePaper.aspx
14. Dye, L. (2014). School counselors’ activities in predominantly African
American schools: an exploratory study. Michigan Journal of
Counseling, 41(1), 18-37. doi:10.22237/mijoc/1393632120
15. Evans, G. W., & Kim, P. (2012). Childhood poverty, chronic stress,
self-regulation, and coping. Child Development Perspective, 7(1), 43-
48. doi:https://doiorg.ezproxy.liberty.edu/10.1111/cdep.12013
16. Gabrielli, J., Gill, M., Koester, L. S. and Borntrager, C. (2014).
Psychological perspectives on ‘acute on chronic’ trauma in children:
Implications of the 2010 earthquake in Haiti. Child & Society, 28: 438-
450. doi:10.1111/chso.12010
17. Gonzalez-Gonzalez, N. L., Suarez, M. N., Perez-Pinero, B., Armas, H.,
Domenech, E. M., & Bartha, J. L. (2006). Persistence of fetal memory
into neonatal life. Acta Obstetricia et Gynecologica
Scandinavica, 85(10), 1160-1164.
doi:https://doi.org/10.1080/00016340600855854
18. Hair, N. L., Hanson, J. L., Wolfe, B. L., & Pollak, S. D. (2015).
Association of child poverty, brain development, and academic
achievement. JAMA Pediatrics, 169(9), 822–829.
doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.1475
European Journal of Educational Sciences, Special Edition, October 2019, ISSN: 1857- 6036
99
19. Hatcher, A. M., Gibbs, A., Jewkes, R., McBride, R., Peacock, D., &
Christofides, N. (2019). Effect of childhood poverty and trauma on
adult depressive symptoms among young men in Peri-Urban South
African settlements. Journal of Adolescent Health, 64(1), 79-85.
doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2018.07.026
20. Hendricks, E. A., & Tanga, P. T. (2019). Effects of bullying on the
psychological functioning of victims. Southern African Journal of
Social Work & Social Development, 31(1), 1–17. https://doi-
org.ezproxy.liberty.edu/10.25159/2415-5829/3939
21. Herman, J. (2001). Trauma and recovery: The aftermath of violence –
from domestic violence to political terror. New York, NY: Basic
Books.
22. Herman, J. (2012). CPTSD is a distinct entity: Comment on Resick et
al. (2012). Journal of Traumatic Stress, 25, 256-257.
doi:10.1002/jts.21697
23. James, D. K. (2010). Fetal learning: a critical review. Infant & Child
Development, 19(1), 45–54. https://doi-
org.ezproxy.liberty.edu/10.1002/icd.653
24. Johnson, B. C. (2012). Aftercare for survivors of human trafficking.
Social Work and Christianity, 39(4), 370-389.
25. Kim, S. Fonagy, P., Allen, J., & Strathearn, L. (2014). Mothers’
unresolved trauma blunts amygdala response to infant distress, Social
Neuroscience, 9:4, 352-363. 10.1080/17470919.2014.896287
26. Kiser, L. J., & Black, M. M. (2005). Family processes in the midst of
urban poverty: What does the trauma literature tell us? Aggression and
Violent Behavior, 10(6), 715-750.
doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2005.02.003
27. Klest, B. (2012). Childhood trauma, poverty, and adult victimization.
Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 4(3),
245-251. http://dx.doi.org.ezproxy.liberty.edu/10.1037/a0024468
28. Ladd, H. F. (2012). Education and poverty: Confronting the evidence.
Journal of Policy Analysis and Management, 31(2) 203-227.
doi:10.1002/pam.21615
29. Levers, L. L. (2012). Trauma counseling: Theories and interventions
(p. 496). New York, NY:
Springer Publishing Company.
30. Lowdermilk, E., & Brunache, J. (2013). Experiencing unexpected
pathways: A grounded theory study of the surprising transformation of
inner-city youth. Social Work and Christianity, 40(3), 322-351.
31. Matsko, K. K., & Hammerness, K. (2014). Unpacking the “urban” in
urban teacher education: Making a case for context-specific
preparation. Journal of Teacher Education, 65(2), 128-144. doi:
10.1177/0022487113511645
European Journal of Educational Sciences, Special Edition, October 2019, ISSN: 1857- 6036
100
32. Matthews, K. A., Jennings, J. R., Lee, L., & Pardini, D. A. (2017).
Bullying and being bullied in childhood are associated with different
psychosocial risk factors for poor physical health in men. Association
for Psychological Science, 28(6), 808-821.
doi:10.1177/0956797617697700
33. McAdams, C. R., & Schmidt, C. D. (2007). How to help a bully:
Recommendations for counseling the proactive aggressor.
Professional School Counseling, 11(2), 120-128. Retrieved from
https://www.schoolcounselor.org/asca/media/asca/home/McAdamsV
ol11No2-(1)
34. Myers, H. F., Wyatt, G. E., Ullman, J. B., Loeb, T. B., Chin, D., Prause,
N., . . . Liu, H. (2015). Cumulative burden of lifetime adversities:
Trauma and mental health in low-SES African Americans and
Latino/as. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and
Policy, 7(3), 243-251.
http://dx.doi.org.ezproxy.liberty.edu/10.1037/a0039077
35. Neuburger, S., Jansen, P., Heil, M., & Quaiser-Pohl, C. (2012). A
threat in the classroom: Gender stereotype activation and mental-
rotation performance in elementary-school children. Zeitschrift fur
Psychologie, 220(2), 61-69. doi:10.1027/2151-2604/a000097
36. Patrick, P. D., Lipina, Colombo, S. J., & Jorge, A. (2010). Poverty and
brain development during childhood: An approach from cognitive
psychology and neuroscience. Journal of the International
Neuropsychological Society, 16(3), 582-584.
doi:http://dx.doi.org.ezproxy.liberty.edu/10.1017/S135561771000039
1
37. Payne, R. K. (1996). A framework for understanding poverty (4th ed.,
p. 75). Highlands, TX: aha! Process, Inc.
38. Shamai, M. (2018). Is poverty a collective trauma? A joint learning
process with women living in poverty in the city of Haifa in Israel. The
British Journal of Social Work, 48(6), 1718–1735.
https://doi.org/10.1093/bjsw/bcx116
39. Skaine, R. (2015). Abuse: An encyclopedia of causes, consequences,
and treatments (p. 40). Santa Barbara, CA: Greenwood.
40. Stebnicki, M. A. (2017). Disaster mental health counseling:
Responding to trauma in a multicultural context. New York, NY:
Springer Publishing Company.
41. Steele, W., & Malchiodi, C. A. (2012). Trauma-Informed practices
with children and Adolescents (p. 15). New York, NY: Routledge.
42. Stewart, P. (2014). Suffering fiction in the unnamable. Samuel Beckett
Today/ Aujourd’Hui, 26, 165-177,315.
European Journal of Educational Sciences, Special Edition, October 2019, ISSN: 1857- 6036
101
43. Tariq, Q., & Tayyab, A. (2011). Bullying and its psychological effects
on adult and adolescent students. Journal of Educational Research,
14(2), 22-31.
44. Taylor, L., & Barrett, W. (2018). Developing a trauma-informed
approach to closing the poverty-related attainment gap. Educational
and Child Psychology, 35(3), 64–75.
45. Tucker, C. M., Porter, T., Reinke, W. M., Herman, K. C., Ivery, P. D.,
Mack, C. E., &Jackson, E. S. (2005). Promoting teacher efficacy for
working with culturally diverse students. Preventing School Failure,
50(1), 29-34. doi:https://doi-
org.ezproxy.liberty.edu/10.3200/PSFL.50.1
46. Two-Generation approaches to addressing poverty: A toolkit for state
legislators (2018). In National Conference of State Legislatures, (p. 3).
47. Uhernik, J. A. (2017). Using Neuroscience in trauma therapy, Creative
and compassionate counseling (p. 54) New York, NY: Routledge.
48. Vanderbilt, D., & Augustyn, M. (2010). The effects of bullying.
Paediatrics and Child Health, 20(7), 315-320.
doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paed.2010.03.008
49. van der Veen, R., & Preece, J. (2005) Poverty reduction and adult
education: Beyond basic education, International Journal of Lifelong
Education, 24(5), 381-391. doi: 10.1080/02601370500169723
50. Wagner, K. D. (2016, November 29). Effects of childhood trauma on
depression and suicidality in adulthood. Psychiatric Times, 33(11)
Stressful Life Events and Associations With Child and Family
Emotional and Behavioral Well-Being in Diverse Immigrant and
Refugee Populations
Jerica M. Berge, PhD, MPH, LMFT, Samaria Mountain, BS, Susan Telke, MS,
Amanda Trofholz, MPH, RD, Katie Lingras, PhD, LP, Roli Dwivedi, MD,
and Lisa Zak-Hunter, PhD, LMFT
University of Minnesota Medical School, Minneapolis, Minnesota
Objective: Although stressful life events (SLEs) have been suggested to be associated with
child well-being, few studies have examined SLEs with child and family behavioral and
emotional well-being, especially within diverse populations. The current study examined
the associations between SLEs and child behavioral and emotional outcomes, in addition to
family-level measures of well-being. Method: Children 5–7 years old and their families
(n � 150) from 6 racial and ethnic groups (n � 25 each for African American, Hispanic,
Hmong, Native American, Somali, White families) participated in this mixed-methods
study. Participants were recruited through primary care clinics. Results: Results showed
that all racially and ethnically diverse immigrant and refugee families were experiencing
SLEs. The majority of diverse children were experiencing emotional and behavioral
problems (i.e., hyperactivity, emotional) in the face of SLEs (i.e., combined SLE score,
health-related events), with Somali children being at highest risk. Additionally, the majority
of diverse families did not experience lower family functioning in response the SLEs,
except regarding certain SLEs (i.e., health-related, legal). However, specific families (i.e.,
Somali) experienced lower family functioning in the face of multiple SLEs. Discussion:
Health care practitioners should consider screening and providing extra resources for
reducing stress in children, given all children in the study had some emotional and
behavioral problems in the face of SLEs. Additionally, it would be important for practi-
tioners to know which families are at greatest risk for experiencing SLEs (i.e., African
American, Native American, Somali families) to ensure they are provided with the re-
sources necessary to mitigate the impact of SLEs.
Public Significance Statement
This study investigated Stressful Life Events (SLEs) in racially/ethnically diverse popula-
tions. SLEs were prevalent and were associated with lower levels of emotional and
behavioral well-being in some children. Many families experienced high levels of family
functioning, despite having SLEs, which may provide potential intervention targets.
This article was published Online First August 27, 2020.
X Jerica M. Berge, PhD, MPH, LMFT, Samaria Mountain,
BS, X Susan Telke, MS, and X Amanda Trofholz, MPH, RD,
Department of Family Medicine and Community Health, Uni-
versity of Minnesota Medical School, Minneapolis, Minnesota;
Katie Lingras, PhD, LP, Department of Psychiatry, University of
Minnesota Medical School, Minneapolis, Minnesota; Roli
Dwivedi, MD and X Lisa Zak-Hunter, PhD, LMFT, Depart-
ment of Family Medicine and Community Health, University of
Minnesota Medical School, Minneapolis, Minnesota.
Research is supported by National Heart, Lung, and
Blood Institute Grant R01HL126171 (principal investiga-
tor: Jerica M. Berge, PhD, MPH, LMFT). The content is
solely the responsibility of the authors and does not nec-
essarily represent the official views of the National Heart,
Lung and Blood Institute or the National Institutes of
Health. Authors have no financial disclosures or conflicts
of interest to report.
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-
dressed to Jerica M. Berge, PhD, MPH, LMFT, Depart-
ment of Family Medicine and Community Health, Univer-
sity of Minnesota Medical School, 717 Delaware Street
Southeast, Room 425, Minneapolis, MN 55414. E-mail:
jberge@umn.edu
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
Ps
yc
ho
lo
gi
ca
l
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
on
e
of
its
al
lie
d
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
Families, Systems, & Health
© 2020 American Psychological Association 2020, Vol. 38, No. 4, 380–395
ISSN: 1091-7527 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/fsh0000524
380
https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3371-351X
https://orcid.org/0000-0003-0238-7248
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-2759-8220
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0065-5168
mailto:jberge@umn.edu
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/fsh0000524
Keywords: stressful life events, emotional well-being, behavioral problems, diverse
families, immigrants/refugees
Stressful life events (SLEs) are discrete,
acute stressors that disrupt an individual’s usual
activities and typically result in substantial
change and readjustment (Cronholm et al.,
2015; Manyema, Norris, & Richter, 2018;
Neece, Green, & Baker, 2012; Thoits, 2010).
Some examples of SLEs include death, financial
problems (e.g., job loss), interpersonal relation-
ship problems (e.g., divorce, separation), health
problems (e.g., diagnosis of a disease, exacer-
bation of a disease, mental health condition),
trauma (e.g., violence, sexual), and legal prob-
lems (e.g., prison, jail time, problems with the
police). SLEs and adverse childhood events
(ACEs) are similar in that the event provoking
the stress may be the same (e.g., divorce of a
parent, sexual trauma); however, ACEs, by defi-
nition, occur in childhood (�18 years), whereas
SLEs may occur across the life span (Allen,
Rapee, & Sandberg, 2008; Cronholm et al., 2015;
Felitti, 1993; Felitti, 2019; Felitti et al., 2019; Ge,
Natsuaki, & Conger, 2006; Manyema et al., 2018;
Thoits, 2010).
Prior research has shown that SLEs are asso-
ciated with negative health and well-being out-
comes at the individual level, especially with
children. For example, several cross-sectional
studies have shown that SLEs (i.e., death, di-
vorce, legal, health, financial) are associated
with emotional and behavioral problems in chil-
dren and adolescents including, depression,
anxiety, disordered eating behaviors, child ad-
justment problems, and reduced coping skills in
response to stress (Allen et al., 2008; Berge,
Loth, Hanson, Croll-Lampert, & Neumark-
Sztainer, 2012; Ge et al., 2006; Harland, Reij-
neveld, Brugman, Verloove-Vanhorick, & Ver-
hulst, 2002; Loth, van den Berg, Eisenberg, &
Neumark-Sztainer, 2008; Siegel et al., 1992). In
addition, longitudinal studies have found that
parental divorce was associated with more prob-
lematic child behavior (e.g., conduct disorder)
and lower emotional well-being (e.g., depres-
sive symptoms, anxiety) into adolescence and
adulthood (Conger et al., 2002; Ge et al., 2006;
Thoits, 2010). Combined, these studies have
suggested that SLEs can have immediate nega-
tive influences on child health and wellness and
long-term impacts because they track into ado-
lescence and adulthood. Researchers have also
shown that SLEs (i.e., divorce, financial, legal)
are associated with negative outcomes at the
family level, such as lower family functioning,
increased family conflict and interpersonal prob-
lems, and reduced familial well-being (Allen et
al., 2008; Conger et al., 2002; Kaczmarek &
Trambacz-Oleszak, 2017; Manyema et al., 2018;
Neece et al., 2012; Thoits, 2010). Whereas most
of the prior research on SLEs has focused primar-
ily on either the individual (e.g., child) or the
family (e.g., family functioning) level, the current
study investigated the influence of SLEs both at
the individual child level and the family level.
In addition, few studies examining SLEs
have been conducted with families across di-
verse backgrounds (Cronholm et al., 2015).
This may be particularly important because
families from diverse backgrounds may be at
higher risk of experiencing SLEs. For example,
some studies have shown African American and
Hispanic families report higher levels of ACEs,
discrimination, and harassment, which have
been shown to be associated with mental health
problems (e.g., depression, disordered eating
behaviors, low self-esteem, substance abuse;
Ellis, MacDonald, Lincoln, & Cabral, 2008;
Lee & Chang, 2012; Neumark-Sztainer et al.,
2002; Ortega, Rosenheck, Alegría, & Desai,
2000; van den Berg, Mond, Eisenberg, Ackard,
& Neumark-Sztainer, 2010) and chronic disease
(e.g., diabetes, obesity, hypertension, cardiovas-
cular disease; Arcan et al., 2014; Cooper et al.,
2000; Krieger, Kosheleva, Waterman, Chen, &
Koenen, 2011; Mensah, Mokdad, Ford, Green-
lund, & Croft, 2005). In addition, immigration
and citizenship have also been shown to be asso-
ciated with health disparities (Escobar, Hoyos
Nervi, & Gara, 2000; Gee, Ryan, Laflamme, &
Holt, 2006; Grant et al., 2004). Furthermore, the
sociopolitical climate in the United States has
evolved from a climate of relative acceptance of
racially and ethnically diverse immigrant and ref-
ugee populations to one of nonacceptance
(Abraído-Lanza, Armbrister, Flórez, & Aguirre,
2006; Guo, 2016; Siddiqui, 2016). These sociopo-
litical shifts may also increase SLEs experiences
381STRESSFUL LIFE EVENTS IN DIVERSE FAMILIES
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
Ps
yc
ho
lo
gi
ca
l
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
on
e
of
its
al
lie
d
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
by racially and ethnically diverse families (Hoyt,
Zeiders, Chaku, Toomey & Nair, 2018; Williams
& Medlock, 2017). Therefore, the current study
investigated the prevalence of SLEs and their as-
sociation with emotional and behavioral well-
being outcomes in racially and ethnically diverse
immigrant and refugee populations.
Family systems theory (FST; Whitchurch &
Constantine, 1993; White, Klein, & Martin,
2015) supports the prior research findings de-
scribed above showing that SLEs are influential
at both the individual and family levels. Specif-
ically, one tenet of FST is that events or behav-
iors experienced in one family member influ-
ence other family members, for worse or for
better. For example, if a parent gets divorced
and then experiences depression, it is highly
likely that the child will be affected by the
parent’s symptoms of depression either through
less parental attention or care or by experienc-
ing symptoms of depression themselves. In ad-
dition, FST asserts that it is important to take
into account cultural aspects of one’s family
system when trying to understand how SLEs are
experienced and ultimately impact individual
well-being and family functioning. For exam-
ple, if divorce or death occurs, some cultures
may have traditions or rituals that help them
adjust to such changes in ways that are healthy
versus unhealthy, and these responses may be
driven by cultural values or beliefs.
Thus, using FST as a framework, the main
research questions addressed in the current
study include (a) What is the prevalence of
SLEs in White, African American, Latino, Na-
tive American, Hmong, and Somali families?
(b) What types of child emotional and behav-
ioral problems and levels of family functioning
are described in White, African American, La-
tino, Native American, Hmong, and Somali
families? (c) What is the association between
SLEs and child emotional and behavioral well-
being? and (d) What is the association between
SLEs and family functioning? Results from this
study will provide information about the influ-
ence of SLEs at the individual and family lev-
els, in addition to how SLEs are experienced
across different racial and ethnic immigrant and
refugee groups. Findings may be useful for
practitioners who work with families (e.g., men-
tal health workers, physicians) in addition to
future intervention research.
Method
Data for the current study are from Phase 1 of
Family Matters (Berge et al., 2017), a National
Institutes of Health–funded observational study
designed to identify novel risk and protective
factors for childhood obesity in the home envi-
ronments of racially and ethnically diverse and
primarily low-income children (n � 150). Phase
1 of the Family Matters study included an in-
depth observational study of diverse families
using a variety of methods (in-home observa-
tions, 24-hr dietary recalls, individual inter-
views, surveys), with the goal of using these
observations to inform the development of a
culturally appropriate survey to be conducted
within a larger sample of families (n � 1,200)
during Phase 2, as well as subsequent interven-
tions geared toward families of elementary
school-age children. In-depth details about both
study phases are published elsewhere (Berge et
al., 2017). The University of Minnesota’s Insti-
tutional Review Board Human Subjects Com-
mittee approved all protocols used in the Family
Matters study; all adult participants provided
written informed consent and parental consent
for their children, and all children assented to
the study.
Participants
The study recruited children and their fami-
lies attending family medicine clinics in the
Minneapolis–Saint Paul, Minnesota, area be-
tween 2015 and 2016 via a letter sent to them by
their family physician. Children were eligible to
participate in the study if they were between 5
and 7 years old, lived with their parent or pri-
mary guardian more than 50% of the time, and
were from one of six racial and ethnic groups
(non-Hispanic White, non-Hispanic Black, La-
tino, Native American, Hmong, or Somali).
This highly diverse sample was intentionally
recruited because children from racially and
ethnically diverse households experience obe-
sity and health disparities at a much higher rate
than do their nondiverse counterparts, and it is
important to identify modifiable factors to mit-
igate obesity risk in childhood. The study im-
plemented a stratified sampling strategy; within
each of the six racial and ethnic groups (n � 25
families from each racial or ethnic group), half
(n � 75) of the sample children recruited had a
382 BERGE ET AL.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
Ps
yc
ho
lo
gi
ca
l
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
on
e
of
its
al
lie
d
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
body mass index (BMI) �85 percentile,
whereas the other half had a BMI between 5
percentile and 85 percentile. In-depth details
regarding recruitment and study design are pub-
lished elsewhere (Berge et al., 2017).
Parents and guardians were mostly mothers
(91%) and were approximately 34.5 years old
(SD � 7.1). Over half of the primary guardians
(58%) were born in the United States. The educa-
tional status was as follows: 40% of parents fin-
ished high school or obtained a general equiva-
lency diploma but had not attended college. About
half (42%) of parents were working full time, and
the majority (70%) of participants reported that
their annual income was �$35,000. The majority
of participants (66%) were receiving public assis-
tance.
Procedures and Data Collection
Eligible families participated in two in-home
visits that were 10 days apart. The current study
uses data from the first home visit, which in-
cluded a parent online survey. Researchers con-
ducted in-home visits in the family’s preferred
language (English, Somali, Spanish, or
Hmong). Participants were paired with a re-
search staff who was both bilingual and bicul-
tural to ensure participant comfort as well as
data collection accuracy.
Measures
Table 1 describes the measures used in the
current study (e.g., parent report of SLEs, child
emotional and behavioral problems, family
functioning).
Statistical Analysis
Descriptive analyses were performed to char-
acterize the sample of 149 families (one family
was dropped from analyses because it did not
complete the survey) with means and standard
deviations across race for number of SLEs,
child behaviors (Strengths and Difficulties
Questionnaire subscales; Goodman, 2001), and
family functioning and to evaluate modeling
assumptions. Linear regression with Huber–
White robust standard errors (to protect against
possible misspecification of the error structure)
were used to estimate means with 95% confi-
dence intervals and p values. Adjusted models
controlled for child gender, parent gender, edu-
cation, mental health (depression, anxiety),
household income, number of children in the
household, and time in the United States. Mod-
els were fitted separately for each scale or sub-
scale measure of child behavior, family func-
tioning, and the occurrence of SLEs. All
analyses were performed in Stata 15.1 SE
(www.statacorp.com).
Results
Results of the current study are presented
below by research question.
Research Question 1: What is the preva-
lence of SLEs in White, African American,
Latino, Native American, Hmong, and So-
mali families?
Overall, families reported experiencing be-
tween one and two SLEs in the prior 6 months.
In addition, there were significant differences in
the prevalence of SLEs by race or ethnicity (see
Table 2; p � .007). African American and Na-
tive American families reported the highest
prevalence of SLEs overall. When examined
across the four specific categories of SLEs (i.e.,
health-related, financial, interpersonal, and le-
gal), Native American and African American
families reported the most health-related SLEs,
African American families reported the most
interpersonal SLEs, and Hmong families re-
ported the most financial and legal SLEs.
Research Question 2: What types of child
emotional and behavioral problems and
levels of family functioning are described
in White, African American, Latino, Na-
tive American, Hmong, and Somali
families?
Across races, there were significant differ-
ences in average child emotional and behavioral
problems as measured by the Strengths and
Difficulties Questionnaire subscales, including
emotional problems (p � .01) and peer prob-
lems (see Table 2; p � .001). Specifically,
White families reported the lowest scores and
Native Americans reported the highest scores
for both emotional problems and peer problems.
There were also significant differences in aver-
age prosocial (e.g., helping others) scores across
race and ethnicity (p � .004), with higher
prosocial scores for White and lower scores for
383STRESSFUL LIFE EVENTS IN DIVERSE FAMILIES
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
Ps
yc
ho
lo
gi
ca
l
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
on
e
of
its
al
lie
d
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
https://www.statacorp.com
T
ab
le
1
M
ea
su
re
s
U
se
d
in
th
e
C
ur
re
nt
A
na
ly
si
s
F
ro
m
th
e
F
am
il
y
M
at
te
rs
st
ud
y
C
on
st
ru
ct
Q
ue
st
io
n
R
es
po
ns
e
op
tio
ns
O
pe
ra
tio
na
liz
ed
va
ri
ab
le
St
re
ss
fu
l
lif
e
ev
en
ts
St
re
ss
fu
l
lif
e
ev
en
ts
(B
ru
gh
a
&
C
ra
gg
,
19
90
)
“H
av
e
an
y
of
th
e
fo
llo
w
in
g
lif
e
ev
en
ts
or
pr
ob
le
m
s
ha
pp
en
ed
to
yo
u
du
ri
ng
th
e
la
st
6
m
on
th
s?
”
1.
Y
es
,
an
d
I
st
il
l
th
in
k
ab
ou
t
it
a
lo
t
2.
Y
es
,
an
d
I
st
il
l
th
in
k
ab
ou
t
it
a
li
tt
le
3.
Y
es
,
bu
t
I
do
no
t
th
in
k
ab
ou
t
it
4.
N
o
If
a
pa
rt
ic
ip
an
t
se
le
ct
ed
Y
es
(O
pt
io
ns
1,
2,
or
3)
,
th
e
ev
en
t
w
as
co
de
d
as
ha
vi
ng
oc
cu
rr
ed
.
If
N
o
(O
pt
io
n
4)
w
as
se
le
ct
ed
,
th
e
ev
en
t
w
as
co
de
d
as
no
t
ha
vi
ng
oc
cu
rr
ed
.
H
ea
lth
ev
en
ts
“Y
ou
yo
ur
se
lf
su
ff
er
ed
a
se
ri
ou
s
ill
ne
ss
,
in
ju
ry
,
or
an
as
sa
ul
t”
“A
se
ri
ou
s
ill
ne
ss
,
in
ju
ry
,
or
as
sa
ul
t
ha
pp
en
ed
to
a
cl
os
e
re
la
tiv
e”
“Y
ou
r
pa
re
nt
,
ch
ild
or
sp
ou
se
di
ed
”
“A
cl
os
e
fa
m
ily
fr
ie
nd
or
an
ot
he
r
re
la
tiv
e
(a
un
t,
co
us
in
,
gr
an
dp
ar
en
t)
di
ed
”
Fi
na
nc
ia
l
st
re
ss
or
s
“Y
ou
be
ca
m
e
un
em
pl
oy
ed
or
w
er
e
se
ek
in
g
w
or
k
un
su
cc
es
sf
ul
ly
fo
r
m
or
e
th
an
a
m
on
th
”
“Y
ou
w
er
e
fir
ed
or
la
id
of
f
fr
om
yo
ur
jo
b”
“Y
ou
ha
d
a
m
aj
or
fin
an
ci
al
cr
is
is
”
In
te
rp
er
so
na
l
st
re
ss
or
s
“Y
ou
ha
d
a
se
pa
ra
tio
n
du
e
to
m
ar
ita
l
di
ffi
cu
lti
es
”
“Y
ou
br
ok
e
of
f
a
st
ea
dy
re
la
tio
ns
hi
p”
“Y
ou
ha
d
a
se
ri
ou
s
pr
ob
le
m
w
ith
a
cl
os
e
fr
ie
nd
,
ne
ig
hb
or
,
or
re
la
tiv
e”
L
eg
al
ev
en
ts
“Y
ou
ha
d
pr
ob
le
m
s
w
ith
th
e
po
lic
e
an
d
a
co
ur
t
ap
pe
ar
an
ce
”
“S
om
et
hi
ng
yo
u
va
lu
ed
w
as
lo
st
or
st
ol
en
”
384 BERGE ET AL.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
Ps
yc
ho
lo
gi
ca
l
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
on
e
of
its
al
lie
d
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
T
ab
le
1
(c
on
ti
nu
ed
)
C
on
st
ru
ct
Q
ue
st
io
n
R
es
po
ns
e
op
tio
ns
O
pe
ra
tio
na
liz
ed
va
ri
ab
le
Fa
m
ily
fu
nc
tio
ni
ng
Fa
m
ily
fu
nc
tio
ni
ng
(B
yl
es
et
al
.,
19
88
)
“H
ow
st
ro
ng
ly
do
yo
u
ag
re
e
w
ith
th
e
fo
llo
w
in
g
st
at
em
en
ts
ab
ou
t
yo
ur
cu
rr
en
t
ho
m
e?
”
“F
am
ily
m
em
be
rs
ar
e
ac
ce
pt
ed
fo
r
w
ho
th
ey
ar
e”
“W
e
av
oi
d
di
sc
us
si
ng
ou
r
fe
ar
s
an
d
co
nc
er
ns
”
“M
ak
in
g
de
ci
si
on
s
is
a
pr
ob
le
m
fo
r
th
e
fa
m
ily
”
“W
e
do
n’
t
ge
t
al
on
g
w
el
l
to
ge
th
er
”
“W
e
ca
n
ex
pr
es
s
fe
el
in
gs
to
ea
ch
ot
he
r”
“P
la
nn
in
g
fa
m
ily
ac
tiv
iti
es
is
di
ffi
cu
lt
be
ca
us
e
w
e
m
is
un
de
rs
ta
nd
ea
ch
ot
he
r”
“I
n
tim
es
of
cr
is
is
w
e
ca
n
tu
rn
to
ea
ch
ot
he
r
fo
r
su
pp
or
t”
“W
e
ca
nn
ot
ta
lk
to
ea
ch
ot
he
r
ab
ou
t
th
e
sa
dn
es
s
w
e
fe
el
”
“T
he
re
ar
e
lo
ts
of
ba
d
fe
el
in
gs
in
th
e
fa
m
ily
”
“W
e
ar
e
ab
le
to
m
ak
e
de
ci
si
on
s
ab
ou
t
ho
w
to
so
lv
e
pr
ob
le
m
s”
“W
e
co
nfi
de
in
ea
ch
ot
he
r
(b
y
“c
on
fid
e”
w
e
m
ea
n
to
tr
us
t
yo
ur
fa
m
ily
m
em
be
rs
en
ou
gh
to
te
ll
th
em
so
m
et
hi
ng
th
at
is
im
po
rt
an
t
to
yo
u)
”
1.
St
ro
ng
ly
di
sa
gr
ee
2.
So
m
ew
ha
t
di
sa
gr
ee
3.
So
m
ew
ha
t
ag
re
e
4.
St
ro
ng
ly
ag
re
e
Su
m
sc
or
e
of
L
ik
er
t
sc
al
e
w
ith
re
ve
rs
e
co
di
ng
fo
r
•
W
e
av
oi
d
di
sc
us
si
ng
ou
r
fe
ar
s
an
d
co
nc
er
ns
•
M
ak
in
g
de
ci
si
on
s
is
a
pr
ob
le
m
fo
r
th
e
fa
m
ily
•
W
e
do
n’
t
ge
t
al
on
g
w
el
l
to
ge
th
er
•
Pl
an
ni
ng
fa
m
ily
ac
tiv
iti
es
is
di
ffi
cu
lt
be
ca
us
e
w
e
m
is
un
de
rs
ta
nd
ea
ch
ot
he
r
•
W
e
ca
nn
ot
ta
lk
to
ea
ch
ot
he
r
ab
ou
t
th
e
sa
dn
es
s
w
e
fe
el
•
T
he
re
ar
e
lo
ts
of
ba
d
fe
el
in
gs
in
th
e
fa
m
ily
H
ig
he
r
sc
or
es
in
di
ca
te
hi
gh
er
fa
m
ily
fu
nc
tio
ni
ng
.
C
hi
ld
be
ha
vi
or
St
re
ng
th
s
an
d
D
if
fic
ul
tie
s
Q
ue
st
io
nn
ai
re
su
bs
ca
le
s
(G
oo
dm
an
,
20
01
)
“B
as
ed
on
[c
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
’s
be
ha
vi
or
ov
er
th
e
la
st
SI
X
M
O
N
T
H
S,
pl
ea
se
re
sp
on
d
to
th
e
fo
llo
w
in
g
qu
es
tio
ns
.”
1.
N
ot
tr
ue
2.
So
m
ew
ha
t
tr
ue
3.
C
er
ta
in
ly
tr
ue
C
on
du
ct
Pr
ob
le
m
s
“[
C
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
of
te
n
lo
se
s
hi
s/
he
r
te
m
pe
r”
“[
C
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
of
te
n
fig
ht
s
w
ith
ot
he
r
ch
ild
re
n
or
bu
lli
es
th
em
”
“[
C
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
is
of
te
n
ar
gu
m
en
ta
tiv
e
w
ith
ad
ul
ts
”
“[
C
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
is
ge
ne
ra
lly
w
el
l-
be
ha
ve
d,
us
ua
lly
do
es
w
ha
t
ad
ul
ts
re
qu
es
t”
“[
C
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
ca
n
be
sp
ite
fu
l
to
ot
he
rs
A
ve
ra
ge
sc
or
e
w
ith
re
ve
rs
e
co
di
ng
fo
r
[c
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
is
ge
ne
ra
lly
w
el
l-
be
ha
ve
d,
us
ua
lly
do
es
w
ha
t
ad
ul
ts
re
qu
es
t”
H
ig
he
r
sc
or
es
in
di
ca
te
hi
gh
er
co
nd
uc
t
pr
ob
le
m
s.
(t
ab
le
co
nt
in
ue
s)
385STRESSFUL LIFE EVENTS IN DIVERSE FAMILIES
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
Ps
yc
ho
lo
gi
ca
l
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
on
e
of
its
al
lie
d
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
T
ab
le
1
(c
on
ti
nu
ed
)
C
on
st
ru
ct
Q
ue
st
io
n
R
es
po
ns
e
op
tio
ns
O
pe
ra
tio
na
liz
ed
va
ri
ab
le
E
m
ot
io
na
l
Pr
ob
le
m
s
“[
C
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
of
te
n
co
m
pl
ai
ns
of
he
ad
ac
he
s,
st
om
ac
h-
ac
he
s,
or
si
ck
ne
ss
”
“[
C
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
ha
s
m
an
y
w
or
ri
es
or
of
te
n
se
em
s
w
or
ri
ed
”
“[
C
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
is
of
te
n
un
ha
pp
y,
de
pr
es
se
d,
or
te
ar
fu
l”
“[
C
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
is
ne
rv
ou
s
or
cl
in
gy
in
ne
w
si
tu
at
io
ns
,
ea
si
ly
lo
se
s
co
nfi
de
nc
e”
“[
C
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
ha
s
m
an
y
fe
ar
s,
ea
si
ly
sc
ar
ed
”
A
ve
ra
ge
sc
or
e.
H
ig
he
r
sc
or
es
in
di
ca
te
hi
gh
er
em
ot
io
na
l
pr
ob
le
m
s.
H
yp
er
ac
tiv
ity
“[
C
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
is
re
st
le
ss
,
ov
er
ac
tiv
e,
ca
nn
ot
st
ay
st
ill
fo
r
lo
ng
”
“[
C
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
is
co
ns
ta
nt
ly
fid
ge
tin
g
or
sq
ui
rm
in
g”
“[
C
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
is
ea
si
ly
di
st
ra
ct
ed
,
co
nc
en
tr
at
io
n
w
an
de
rs
”
“[
C
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
ca
n
st
op
an
d
th
in
k
th
in
gs
ou
t
be
fo
re
ac
tin
g”
“[
C
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
ha
s
a
go
od
at
te
nt
io
n
sp
an
,
se
es
w
or
k
th
ro
ug
h
to
th
e
en
d”
A
ve
ra
ge
sc
or
e
w
ith
re
ve
rs
e
co
di
ng
fo
r
•
“[
C
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
ca
n
st
op
an
d
th
in
k
th
in
gs
ou
t
be
fo
re
ac
tin
g”
•
“[
C
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
ha
s
a
go
od
at
te
nt
io
n
sp
an
,
se
es
w
or
k
th
ro
ug
h
to
th
e
en
d”
H
ig
he
r
sc
or
es
in
di
ca
te
hi
gh
er
hy
pe
ra
ct
iv
ity
.
Pe
er
Pr
ob
le
m
s
“[
C
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
ha
s
at
le
as
t
on
e
go
od
fr
ie
nd
”
“[
C
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
is
ge
ne
ra
lly
lik
ed
by
ot
he
r
ch
ild
re
n”
“[
C
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
is
pi
ck
ed
on
or
bu
lli
ed
by
ot
he
r
ch
ild
re
n”
“[
C
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
ge
ts
al
on
g
be
tte
r
w
ith
ad
ul
ts
th
an
w
ith
ot
he
r
ch
ild
re
n”
A
ve
ra
ge
sc
or
e
w
ith
re
ve
rs
e
co
di
ng
fo
r
•
“[
C
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
ha
s
at
le
as
t
on
e
go
od
fr
ie
nd
”
•
“[
C
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
is
ge
ne
ra
lly
lik
ed
by
ot
he
r
ch
ild
re
n”
H
ig
he
r
sc
or
es
in
di
ca
te
hi
gh
er
pe
er
pr
ob
le
m
s.
Pr
os
oc
ia
l
“[
C
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
is
co
ns
id
er
at
e
of
ot
he
r
pe
op
le
’s
fe
el
in
gs
”
“[
C
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
sh
ar
es
re
ad
ily
w
ith
ot
he
r
ch
ild
re
n,
fo
r
ex
am
pl
e
to
ys
,
tr
ea
ts
,
pe
nc
ils
”
“[
C
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
is
he
lp
fu
l
if
so
m
eo
ne
is
hu
rt
,
up
se
t
or
fe
el
in
g
ill
”
“[
C
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
is
ki
nd
to
yo
un
ge
r
ch
ild
re
n”
“[
C
hi
ld
’s
na
m
e]
of
te
n
of
fe
rs
to
he
lp
ot
he
rs
(p
ar
en
ts
,
te
ac
he
rs
,
ot
he
r
ch
ild
re
n)
”
A
ve
ra
ge
sc
or
e.
H
ig
he
r
sc
or
es
in
di
ca
te
hi
gh
er
pr
os
oc
ia
l
be
ha
vi
or
.
386 BERGE ET AL.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
Ps
yc
ho
lo
gi
ca
l
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
on
e
of
its
al
lie
d
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
T
ab
le
1
(c
on
ti
nu
ed
)
C
on
st
ru
ct
Q
ue
st
io
n
R
es
po
ns
e
op
tio
ns
O
pe
ra
tio
na
liz
ed
va
ri
ab
le
C
on
tr
ol
va
ri
ab
le
s
Pa
re
nt
ed
uc
at
io
n
“W
ha
t
is
th
e
hi
gh
es
t
gr
ad
e
or
ye
ar
of
sc
ho
ol
th
at
Y
O
U
ha
ve
co
m
pl
et
ed
?”
1.
M
id
dl
e
sc
ho
ol
or
ju
ni
or
hi
gh
2.
So
m
e
hi
gh
sc
ho
ol
3.
H
ig
h
sc
ho
ol
or
G
E
D
4.
V
oc
at
io
na
l,
te
ch
ni
ca
l,
tr
ad
e
or
ot
he
r
ce
rt
ifi
ca
ti
on
pr
og
ra
m
5.
A
ss
oc
ia
te
de
gr
ee
6.
B
ac
he
lo
r
de
gr
ee
7.
G
ra
du
at
e
or
pr
of
es
si
on
al
de
gr
ee
(M
S,
M
B
A
,
M
D
,
P
hD
,
et
c.
)
8.
O
th
er
Fa
ct
or
sc
or
e
H
ou
se
ho
ld
in
co
m
e
“W
ha
t
is
yo
ur
ye
ar
ly
T
O
T
A
L
H
O
U
SE
H
O
L
D
in
co
m
e?
(i
.e
.,
in
co
m
e
fr
om
A
L
L
fa
m
ily
m
em
be
rs
w
ho
se
jo
b
he
lp
s
su
pp
or
t
th
e
fa
m
ily
)”
“T
hi
s
in
cl
ud
es
w
ag
es
,
ca
sh
as
si
st
an
ce
,
So
ci
al
Se
cu
ri
ty
,
ch
ild
su
pp
or
t,
et
c.
”
1.
L
es
s
th
an
$2
0,
00
0
2.
$2
0,
00
0–
$3
4,
99
9
3.
$3
5,
00
0–
$4
9,
99
9
4.
$5
0,
00
0–
$7
4,
99
9
5.
$7
5,
00
0–
$9
9,
99
9
6.
$1
00
,0
00
or
m
or
e
Fa
ct
or
sc
or
e
H
ou
se
ho
ld
ra
ce
“W
hi
ch
ra
ci
al
/e
th
ni
c
gr
ou
p
be
st
de
sc
ri
be
s
yo
ur
fa
m
ily
?”
W
hi
te
A
fr
ic
an
A
m
er
ic
an
H
m
on
g
So
m
al
i
N
at
iv
e
A
m
er
ic
an
L
at
in
o
Pa
rt
ic
ip
an
ts
se
lf
-i
de
nt
ifi
ed
w
hi
ch
ra
ci
al
/e
th
ni
c
gr
ou
p
be
st
de
sc
ri
be
d
th
ei
r
ho
us
eh
ol
d.
N
o.
of
ch
ild
re
n
in
th
e
ho
us
eh
ol
d
“H
ow
m
an
y
ch
ild
re
n
(y
ou
ng
er
th
an
18
ye
ar
s
ol
d)
ar
e
liv
in
g
in
th
e
ho
m
e?
”
1,
2,
3,
4,
5,
6,
7,
8,
9,
10
�
1,
2,
3,
4�
T
im
e
in
th
e
U
ni
te
d
St
at
es
“H
ow
m
an
y
ye
ar
s
ha
ve
yo
u
liv
ed
in
th
e
U
ni
te
d
St
at
es
?”
�
1
ye
ar
1–
5
ye
ar
s
5–
10
ye
ar
s
10
–2
0
ye
ar
s
20
–3
0
ye
ar
s
30
�
ye
ar
s
Fa
ct
or
sc
or
e
Pa
re
nt
an
xi
et
y
an
d
pa
re
nt
de
pr
es
si
on
“O
ve
r
th
e
pa
st
2
w
ee
ks
,
ho
w
of
te
n
ha
ve
yo
u
be
en
bo
th
er
ed
by
th
e
fo
llo
w
in
g
pr
ob
le
m
s?
”
“N
ot
be
in
g
ab
le
to
st
op
or
co
nt
ro
l
w
or
ry
in
g”
1.
N
ot
at
al
l
2.
Se
ve
ra
l
da
ys
3.
M
or
e
da
ys
th
an
no
t
4.
N
ea
rl
y
ev
er
y
da
y
Fa
ct
or
sc
or
e
“F
ee
lin
g
do
w
n,
de
pr
es
se
d,
or
ho
pe
le
ss
”
Fa
ct
or
sc
or
e
387STRESSFUL LIFE EVENTS IN DIVERSE FAMILIES
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
Ps
yc
ho
lo
gi
ca
l
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
on
e
of
its
al
lie
d
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
Latino families. In addition, across racial and
ethnic groups, there were significant differences
in overall family functioning, with White fam-
ilies reporting higher overall (i.e., 40.36 partic-
ipants [pts]) family functioning and Latino fam-
ilies reporting lower family functioning (i.e.,
35.28 pts; see Table 2; p � .001).
Research Question 3: What is the associ-
ation between SLEs and child emotional
and behavioral problems in White, African
American, Latino, Native American,
Hmong, and Somali families?
There were many significant results in the full
sample (i.e., all races combined) when examin-
ing the associations between SLEs and child
emotional and behavioral problems (see Table
3). For example, there was an average increase
of 0.14 (95% confidence interval [CI: 0.00,
0.27]; effect size � 0.34) in emotional problems
when considering the occurrence of any SLE.
For health-related SLEs, there was an average
increase of 0.15 ([0.02, 0.29]; effect size �
0.36) in conduct problems. The occurrence of a
legal-related SLE resulted in an average in-
crease of 0.24 ([0.04, 0.44]; effect size � 0.57)
in conduct problems and of 0.14 ([0.01, 0.26];
effect size � 0.41) in peer problems.
Overall, Somali children had higher levels of
emotional and behavioral problems scores for
health-related, financial, interpersonal, and legal
SLEs compared to all other races and ethnici-
ties. Specifically, SLEs were positively associ-
ated with conduct problems, emotional prob-
lems, hyperactivity, and peer problems and
negatively associated with prosocial behaviors
for Somali children.
Research Question 4: What is the associ-
ation between SLEs and family function-
ing in White, African American, Latino,
Native American, Hmong, and Somali
families?
Overall, family functioning change scores
were significantly different across racial and
ethnic groups regarding the occurrence of any
SLE (p � .05). For example, Somali families
experiencing any SLE had an average decrease
of 5.18 (95% CI [�8.76, �1.60]; effect size �
�1.00) in family functioning. Regarding spe-
cific SLEs, a health-related SLE resulted in an
average decrease of 4.80 ([�8.40, �1.20]; ef-T
ab
le
2
U
na
dj
us
te
d
M
ea
n
Sc
or
es
an
d
St
an
da
rd
D
ev
ia
ti
on
s
of
St
re
ss
fu
l
L
if
e
E
ve
nt
s,
C
hi
ld
B
eh
av
io
rs
,
an
d
F
am
il
y
F
un
ct
io
ni
ng
A
cr
os
s
R
ac
e/
E
th
ni
ci
ty
Pr
ev
al
en
ce
s
of
SL
E
s,
em
ot
io
na
l
an
d
be
ha
vi
or
al
pr
ob
le
m
s,
an
d
fa
m
ily
fu
nc
tio
ni
ng
H
ou
se
ho
ld
ra
ce
pa
A
fr
ic
an
A
m
er
ic
an
W
hi
te
H
m
on
g
L
at
in
o
N
at
iv
e
A
m
er
ic
an
So
m
al
i
St
re
ss
fu
l
lif
e
ev
en
ts
A
ny
(r
an
ge
�
0–
12
)
2.
33
(1
.9
9)
1.
28
(1
.4
3)
1.
84
(1
.9
7)
1.
08
(1
.4
1)
2.
08
(1
.9
1)
0.
84
(1
.3
4)
.0
07
H
ea
lth
-r
el
at
ed
(r
an
ge
�
0–
4)
1.
13
(1
.0
3)
0.
52
(0
.6
5)
0.
52
(0
.7
7)
0.
52
(0
.7
1)
1.
24
(1
.3
0)
0.
52
(0
.7
7)
.0
2
Fi
na
nc
ia
l
(r
an
ge
�
0–
3)
0.
50
(0
.6
6)
0.
20
(0
.4
1)
0.
80
(1
.1
2)
0.
36
(0
.7
0)
0.
36
(0
.8
6)
0.
08
(0
.2
8)
.0
01
In
te
rp
er
so
na
l
(r
an
ge
�
0–
3)
0.
50
(0
.7
8)
0.
32
(0
.6
3)
0.
24
(0
.6
0)
0.
08
(0
.2
8)
0.
28
(0
.5
4)
0.
20
(0
.5
8)
.0
8
L
eg
al
(r
an
ge
�
0–
2)
0.
21
(0
.4
1)
0.
24
(0
.4
4)
0.
28
(0
.5
4)
0.
12
(0
.3
3)
0.
20
(0
.4
1)
0.
04
(0
.2
0)
.0
6
C
hi
ld
em
ot
io
na
l
an
d
be
ha
vi
or
al
pr
ob
le
m
s
C
on
du
ct
pr
ob
le
m
s
0.
57
(0
.5
0)
0.
34
(0
.3
1)
0.
41
(0
.3
7)
0.
46
(0
.3
5)
0.
67
(0
.4
9)
0.
44
(0
.4
7)
.0
6
E
m
ot
io
na
l
pr
ob
le
m
s
0.
43
(0
.4
7)
0.
17
(0
.2
4)
0.
41
(0
.3
8)
0.
30
(0
.3
5)
0.
44
(0
.4
6)
0.
34
(0
.4
4)
.0
1
H
yp
er
ac
tiv
ity
0.
68
(0
.4
9)
0.
70
(0
.5
7)
0.
65
(0
.3
8)
0.
69
(0
.3
8)
0.
90
(0
.6
6)
0.
57
(0
.4
6)
.5
Pe
er
pr
ob
le
m
s
0.
37
(0
.2
2)
0.
19
(0
.1
8)
0.
52
(0
.3
7)
0.
50
(0
.3
8)
0.
54
(0
.3
5)
0.
39
(0
.3
4)
�
.0
01
Pr
os
oc
ia
l
1.
60
(0
.3
4)
1.
75
(0
.3
2)
1.
66
(0
.3
3)
1.
37
(0
.4
4)
1.
56
(0
.3
7)
1.
45
(0
.4
3)
.0
04
O
ve
ra
ll
fa
m
ily
fu
nc
tio
ni
ng
(r
an
ge
�
23
–4
4)
39
.5
4
(4
.2
5)
40
.3
6
(3
.9
7)
36
.8
4
(4
.5
4)
35
.2
8
(5
.9
1)
39
.4
0
(4
.6
5)
37
.3
6
(5
.8
2)
.0
01
a
Fo
r
bi
va
ri
at
e
te
st
ac
ro
ss
di
ff
er
en
ce
s
am
on
g
ra
ce
s
fo
r
sc
or
es
.
388 BERGE ET AL.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
Ps
yc
ho
lo
gi
ca
l
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
on
e
of
its
al
lie
d
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
T
ab
le
3
A
dj
us
te
d
P
re
di
ct
ed
M
ea
n
Sc
or
e
C
ha
ng
e
an
d
95
%
C
Is
fo
r
F
am
il
y
F
un
ct
io
ni
ng
an
d
C
hi
ld
B
eh
av
io
rs
by
SL
E
O
cc
ur
re
nc
e
A
cr
os
s
an
d
W
it
hi
n
R
ac
e
SL
E
an
d
SD
Q
su
bs
ca
le
a
H
ou
se
ho
ld
ra
ce
pb
A
ll
ra
ce
s
co
m
bi
ne
d
A
fr
ic
an
A
m
er
ic
an
W
hi
te
H
m
on
g
L
at
in
o
N
at
iv
e
A
m
er
ic
an
So
m
al
i
A
ny
SL
E
C
hi
ld
be
ha
vi
or
C
on
du
ct
pr
ob
le
m
s
0.
14
[�
0.
01
,0
.2
8]
�
0.
02
[�
0.
54
,0
.5
0]
�
0.
23
[�
0.
58
,0
.1
2]
�
0.
05
[�
0.
32
,0
.2
1]
�
0.
12
[�
0.
39
,0
.1
5]
0.
39
[0
.1
1,
0.
67
]
0.
58
[0
.3
0,
0.
86
]
�
.0
01
E
m
ot
io
na
l
pr
ob
le
m
s
0.
14
[0
.0
0,
0.
27
]
0.
01
[�
0.
44
,0
.4
6]
�
0.
17
[�
0.
41
,0
.0
6]
0.
15
[�
0.
09
,0
.3
8]
0.
17
[�
0.
11
,0
.4
5]
0.
20
[�
0.
08
,0
.4
9]
0.
34
[0
.0
3,
0.
65
]
.1
H
yp
er
ac
tiv
ity
0.
09
[�
0.
10
,0
.2
8]
�
0.
14
[�
0.
60
,0
.3
2]
�
0.
36
[�
0.
80
,0
.0
8]
�
0.
10
[�
0.
46
,0
.2
5]
0.
08
[�
0.
25
,0
.4
1]
0.
48
[0
.0
5,
0.
90
]
0.
35
[0
.0
3,
0.
68
]
.0
5
Pe
er
pr
ob
le
m
s
0.
03
[�
0.
09
,0
.1
5]
0.
09
[�
0.
10
,0
.2
8]
�
0.
12
[�
0.
34
,0
.1
0]
�
0.
02
[�
0.
27
,0
.2
3]
�
0.
01
[�
0.
33
,0
.3
1]
�
0.
02
[�
0.
29
,0
.2
6]
0.
13
[�
0.
15
,0
.4
1]
.7
Pr
os
oc
ia
l
�
0.
13
[�
0.
28
,0
.0
1]
�
0.
08
[�
0.
49
,0
.3
2]
�
0.
01
[�
0.
28
,0
.2
5]
0.
19
[�
0.
06
,0
.4
4]
�
0.
03
[�
0.
35
,0
.3
0]
�
0.
42
[�
0.
64
,�
0.
20
]
�
0.
47
[�
0.
74
,�
0.
20
]
.0
01
Fa
m
ily
fu
nc
tio
ni
ng
O
ve
ra
ll
fa
m
ily
fu
nc
tio
ni
ng
�
0.
42
[�
2.
03
,1
.1
9]
�
1.
39
[�
4.
41
,1
.6
3]
1.
19
[�
2.
73
,5
.1
0]
1.
98
[�
1.
22
,5
.1
8]
�
1.
85
[�
6.
49
,2
.8
0]
1.
15
[�
2.
54
,4
.8
3]
�
5.
18
[�
8.
76
,�
1.
60
]
.0
5
H
ea
lth
-r
el
at
ed
SL
E
C
hi
ld
be
ha
vi
or
C
on
du
ct
pr
ob
le
m
s
0.
15
[0
.0
2,
0.
29
]
�
0.
05
[�
0.
43
,0
.3
3]
�
0.
11
[�
0.
42
,0
.1
9]
�
0.
10
[�
0.
33
,0
.1
3]
�
0.
24
[�
0.
47
,0
.0
0]
0.
45
[0
.1
8,
0.
72
]
0.
59
[0
.3
1,
0.
87
]
�
.0
01
E
m
ot
io
na
l
pr
ob
le
m
s
0.
11
[�
0.
02
,0
.2
5]
0.
12
[�
0.
22
,0
.4
6]
�
0.
01
[�
0.
25
,0
.2
2]
0.
10
[�
0.
16
,0
.3
6]
�
0.
03
[�
0.
31
,0
.2
5]
0.
09
[�
0.
22
,0
.4
0]
0.
34
[0
.0
3,
0.
65
]
.6
H
yp
er
ac
tiv
ity
0.
14
[�
0.
05
,0
.3
3]
0.
00
[�
0.
38
,0
.3
9]
�
0.
24
[�
0.
70
,0
.2
1]
�
0.
11
[�
0.
43
,0
.2
1]
�
0.
15
[�
0.
48
,0
.1
8]
0.
59
[0
.1
9,
0.
99
]
0.
36
[0
.0
2,
0.
69
]
.0
2
Pe
er
pr
ob
le
m
s
0.
00
[�
0.
11
,0
.1
2]
0.
04
[�
0.
15
,0
.2
3]
�
0.
12
[�
0.
31
,0
.0
7]
0.
01
[�
0.
23
,0
.2
5]
�
0.
20
[�
0.
53
,0
.1
2]
�
0.
04
[�
0.
29
,0
.2
1]
0.
13
[�
0.
16
,0
.4
1]
.6
Pr
os
oc
ia
l
�
0.
10
[�
0.
25
,0
.0
4]
�
0.
02
[�
0.
36
,0
.3
2]
0.
02
[�
0.
26
,0
.3
0]
0.
23
[0
.0
0,
0.
46
]
0.
25
[�
0.
07
,0
.5
7]
�
0.
48
[�
0.
68
,�
0.
28
]
�
0.
47
[�
0.
73
,�
0.
20
]
�
.0
01
Fa
m
ily
fu
nc
tio
ni
ng
O
ve
ra
ll
fa
m
ily
fu
nc
tio
ni
ng
0.
87
[�
0.
88
,2
.6
2]
2.
32
[�
1.
57
,6
.2
2]
0.
72
[�
2.
99
,4
.4
2]
1.
94
[�
1.
26
,5
.1
5]
2.
13
[�
2.
19
,6
.4
4]
1.
42
[�
2.
01
,4
.8
5]
�
4.
80
[�
8.
40
,�
1.
20
]
.0
6
Fi
na
nc
ia
l
SL
E
C
hi
ld
be
ha
vi
or
C
on
du
ct
pr
ob
le
m
s
�
0.
01
[�
0.
17
,0
.1
5]
0.
26
[�
0.
13
,0
.6
5]
�
0.
29
[�
0.
61
,0
.0
4]
0.
07
[�
0.
21
,0
.3
5]
�
0.
11
[�
0.
4,
0.
18
]
�
0.
07
[�
0.
48
,0
.3
4]
0.
07
[�
0.
28
,0
.4
2]
.3
E
m
ot
io
na
l
pr
ob
le
m
s
0.
06
[�
0.
12
,0
.2
5]
0.
09
[�
0.
29
,0
.4
8]
�
0.
23
[�
0.
48
,0
.0
1]
0.
15
[�
0.
11
,0
.4
1]
0.
24
[�
0.
08
,0
.5
6]
0.
24
[�
0.
37
,0
.8
5]
�
0.
41
[�
0.
69
,�
0.
13
]
.0
1
H
yp
er
ac
tiv
ity
0.
00
[�
0.
20
,0
.2
1]
0.
10
[�
0.
31
,0
.5
2]
0.
12
[�
0.
35
,0
.5
9]
�
0.
22
[�
0.
56
,0
.1
2]
0.
21
[�
0.
26
,0
.6
8]
0.
18
[�
0.
46
,0
.8
1]
�
0.
26
[�
0.
69
,0
.1
7]
.0
4
Pe
er
pr
ob
le
m
s
0.
05
[�
0.
08
,0
.1
8]
0.
15
[�
0.
04
,0
.3
4]
0.
07
[�
0.
20
,0
.3
4]
�
0.
02
[�
0.
31
,0
.2
7]
0.
05
[�
0.
22
,0
.3
2]
0.
14
[�
0.
22
,0
.4
9]
�
0.
28
[�
0.
51
,�
0.
06
]
.1
Pr
os
oc
ia
l
�
0.
04
[�
0.
17
,0
.1
0]
�
0.
29
[�
0.
56
,�
0.
03
]
�
0.
05
[�
0.
32
,0
.2
2]
0.
10
[�
0.
15
,0
.3
4]
�
0.
03
[�
0.
35
,0
.2
8]
�
0.
13
[�
0.
44
,0
.1
8]
�
0.
17
[�
0.
44
,0
.1
1]
.4
Fa
m
ily
fu
nc
tio
ni
ng
O
ve
ra
ll
fa
m
ily
fu
nc
tio
ni
ng
�
0.
51
[�
2.
49
,1
.4
6]
�
1.
45
[�
5.
06
,2
.1
5]
�
3.
95
[�
9.
47
,1
.5
6]
0.
65
[�
2.
97
,4
.2
8]
�
1.
10
[�
5.
08
,2
.8
8]
0.
98
[�
1.
52
,3
.4
7]
�
1.
72
[�
9.
37
,5
.9
3]
.5
(t
ab
le
co
nt
in
ue
s)
389STRESSFUL LIFE EVENTS IN DIVERSE FAMILIES
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
Ps
yc
ho
lo
gi
ca
l
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
on
e
of
its
al
lie
d
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
T
ab
le
3
(c
on
ti
nu
ed
)
SL
E
an
d
SD
Q
su
bs
ca
le
a
H
ou
se
ho
ld
ra
ce
pb
A
ll
ra
ce
s
co
m
bi
ne
d
A
fr
ic
an
A
m
er
ic
an
W
hi
te
H
m
on
g
L
at
in
o
N
at
iv
e
A
m
er
ic
an
So
m
al
i
In
te
rp
er
so
na
l
SL
E
C
hi
ld
be
ha
vi
or
C
on
du
ct
pr
ob
le
m
s
0.
09
[�
0.
09
,0
.2
7]
0.
13
[�
0.
29
,0
.5
4]
�
0.
19
[�
0.
50
,0
.1
3]
�
0.
13
[�
0.
41
,0
.1
4]
�
0.
23
[�
0.
76
,0
.2
9]
0.
16
[�
0.
24
,0
.5
6]
0.
73
[0
.2
8,
1.
18
]
.0
01
E
m
ot
io
na
l
pr
ob
le
m
s
0.
17
[�
0.
01
,0
.3
4]
0.
05
[�
0.
29
,0
.3
8]
�
0.
13
[�
0.
37
,0
.1
1]
0.
57
[0
.3
2,
0.
82
]
0.
10
[�
0.
31
,0
.5
]
0.
16
[�
0.
34
,0
.6
7]
0.
36
[�
0.
09
,0
.8
1]
.0
02
H
yp
er
ac
tiv
ity
0.
06
[�
0.
17
,0
.2
9]
0.
04
[�
0.
31
,0
.3
9]
�
0.
30
[�
0.
71
,0
.1
0]
�
0.
23
[�
0.
62
,0
.1
6]
0.
35
[�
0.
12
,0
.8
1]
0.
19
[�
0.
39
,0
.7
8]
0.
65
[0
.1
1,
1.
19
]
.0
2
Pe
er
pr
ob
le
m
s
�
0.
03
[�
0.
16
,0
.0
9]
0.
09
[�
0.
09
,0
.2
8]
�
0.
05
[�
0.
27
,0
.1
6]
�
0.
10
[�
0.
30
,0
.1
0]
�
0.
27
[�
0.
57
,0
.0
3]
�
0.
07
[�
0.
43
,0
.2
9]
�
0.
02
[�
0.
32
,0
.2
8]
.4
Pr
os
oc
ia
l
�
0.
07
[�
0.
23
,0
.0
9]
�
0.
22
[�
0.
52
,0
.0
9]
0.
23
[�
0.
02
,0
.4
7]
0.
16
[�
0.
06
,0
.3
8]
0.
29
[�
0.
23
,0
.8
1]
�
0.
42
[�
0.
75
,�
0.
10
]
�
0.
38
[�
0.
68
,�
0.
09
]
�
.0
01
Fa
m
ily
fu
nc
tio
ni
ng
O
ve
ra
ll
fa
m
ily
fu
nc
tio
ni
ng
�
0.
99
[�
2.
93
,0
.9
4]
�
4.
22
[�
7.
97
,�
0.
47
]
0.
05
[�
4.
40
,4
.5
0]
0.
27
[�
4.
72
,5
.2
5]
1.
13
[�
2.
87
,5
.1
3]
�
1.
89
[�
5.
94
,2
.1
5]
�
2.
15
[�
7.
44
,3
.1
3]
.5
L
eg
al
SL
E
C
hi
ld
be
ha
vi
or
C
on
du
ct
pr
ob
le
m
s
0.
24
[0
.0
4,
0.
44
]
0.
32
[�
0.
16
,0
.8
]
�
0.
08
[�
0.
44
,0
.2
8]
0.
20
[�
0.
12
,0
.5
2]
0.
23
[�
0.
08
,0
.5
5]
0.
72
[0
.2
6,
1.
17
]
0.
42
[0
.1
5,
0.
69
]
.2
E
m
ot
io
na
l
pr
ob
le
m
s
0.
10
[�
0.
05
,0
.2
6]
0.
15
[�
0.
20
,0
.5
0]
�
0.
15
[�
0.
36
,0
.0
6]
0.
25
[�
0.
05
,0
.5
6]
0.
27
[�
0.
06
,0
.6
0]
0.
01
[�
0.
43
,0
.4
4]
0.
56
[0
.3
1,
0.
81
]
.0
02
H
yp
er
ac
tiv
ity
0.
07
[�
0.
17
,0
.3
0]
0.
35
[�
0.
24
,0
.9
4]
�
0.
26
[�
0.
70
,0
.1
7]
�
0.
11
[�
0.
47
,0
.2
4]
0.
24
[�
0.
24
,0
.7
2]
0.
29
[�
0.
32
,0
.9
0]
0.
20
[�
0.
11
,0
.5
2]
.4
Pe
er
pr
ob
le
m
s
0.
14
[0
.0
1,
0.
26
]
0.
33
[0
.1
5,
0.
50
]
�
0.
06
[�
0.
24
,0
.1
3]
0.
20
[�
0.
06
,0
.4
6]
0.
09
[�
0.
35
,0
.5
4]
�
0.
02
[�
0.
37
,0
.3
3]
0.
65
[0
.4
5,
0.
84
]
�
.0
01
Pr
os
oc
ia
l
�
0.
04
[�
0.
19
,0
.1
2]
�
0.
28
[�
0.
65
,0
.0
9]
0.
21
[�
0.
13
,0
.5
4]
0.
21
[�
0.
02
,0
.4
5]
�
0.
15
[�
0.
61
,0
.3
1]
�
0.
44
[�
0.
76
,�
0.
13
]
�
0.
27
[�
0.
50
,�
0.
04
]
.0
03
Fa
m
ily
fu
nc
tio
ni
ng
O
ve
ra
ll
fa
m
ily
fu
nc
tio
ni
ng
�
0.
69
[�
2.
83
,1
.4
4]
�
1.
98
[�
5.
36
,1
.4
1]
�
0.
22
[�
4.
86
,4
.4
3]
3.
47
[�
0.
40
,7
.3
3]
�
5.
03
[�
12
.0
7,
2.
01
]
�
0.
86
[�
3.
59
,1
.8
7]
�
7.
61
[�
10
.3
9,
�
4.
83
]
�
.0
01
N
ot
e.
Sc
or
es
w
er
e
ad
ju
st
ed
fo
r
ch
ild
ge
nd
er
,
pa
re
nt
ge
nd
er
,
ed
uc
at
io
n,
m
en
ta
l
he
al
th
(d
ep
re
ss
io
n,
an
xi
et
y)
,
ho
us
eh
ol
d
in
co
m
e,
nu
m
be
r
of
ch
ild
re
n
in
th
e
ho
us
eh
ol
d,
an
d
tim
e
in
th
e
U
ni
te
d
St
at
es
.
V
al
ue
s
in
bo
ld
ha
ve
p
�
.0
5
(n
ot
ad
ju
st
ed
fo
r
m
ul
tip
le
co
m
pa
ri
so
ns
).
C
I
�
co
nfi
de
nc
e
in
te
rv
al
;
SL
E
�
st
re
ss
fu
l
lif
e
ev
en
t;
SD
Q
�
St
re
ng
th
s
an
d
D
if
fic
ul
tie
s
Q
ue
st
io
nn
ai
re
.
a
Fo
r
ea
ch
su
bs
ca
le
,
ra
ng
e
�
0
–2
.
b
V
al
ue
fo
r
bi
va
ri
at
e
te
st
ac
ro
ss
sc
or
e
di
ff
er
en
ce
s
am
on
g
ra
ce
s.
390 BERGE ET AL.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
Ps
yc
ho
lo
gi
ca
l
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
on
e
of
its
al
lie
d
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
fect size � �1.48) and legal SLEs resulted in
an average decrease of 7.61 ([�10.39, �4.83];
effect size � �0.93) in family functioning
score for Somali families.
Discussion
Results of the current study support and ex-
tend prior research by being one of the first
studies to examine SLEs in diverse immigrant
and refugee families and to examine the asso-
ciation between SLEs at the individual child and
family levels (Thoits, 2010).
Descriptive results showed that the preva-
lence of SLEs was between one and two in
White, Hmong, Somali, African American, La-
tino, and Native American families, with Afri-
can American and Native American families
experiencing the highest prevalence of SLEs
(i.e., two or more). Although one or two SLEs
may seem like a low number, prior research on
ACEs has shown that even one or two ACEs
increased one’s risk of numerous health, behav-
ioral, and social problems throughout the life
span (Felitti, 1993, 2019; Felitti et al., 2019).
Additionally, ACEs and SLEs tend to be cumu-
lative, and the accumulation of ACEs or SLEs
has a strong graded relationship with health risk
(cardiovascular, mental health, weight-related)
and mortality (Felitti, 1993, 2019; Felitti et al.,
2019). Taken together, these findings suggest
that White and non-White groups experience
SLEs and may need resources to reduce their
potential impact, but especially African Amer-
ican, Native American, and Somali groups may
need them.
In addition, descriptive results showed that
child emotional and behavioral problems were
experienced at low to moderate levels, with the
highest occurrence of problems being hyperac-
tivity, conduct disorder, behavioral problems,
and emotional problems occurring among non-
White children. Also, children from non-White
backgrounds experienced higher prevalence of
emotional and behavioral problems; thus, fam-
ilies may also benefit from resources to help
reduce the potential negative impact of these
child emotional and behavioral problems. Fur-
thermore, descriptive results showed that most
families reported relatively high family func-
tioning. Ultimately, this strength may be helpful
for mitigating the potential negative impact of
SLEs. For example, health care providers who
work with families or interventionists who carry
out family-based interventions could leverage
family support as a potential protective factor
when SLEs occur. FST supports these findings,
in that strong family relationships are expected
to be protective during challenging times, ac-
cording to FST.
Results also indicated significant associations
between SLEs and child emotional and behav-
ioral well-being and family functioning overall,
as well as differences by race and ethnicity.
Regarding child emotional and behavioral prob-
lems, the occurrence of any SLE was associated
with higher levels of emotional problems in all
children. This finding supports prior studies
showing SLEs have immediate and long-term
influences on child emotional and behavioral
well-being (Allen et al., 2008; Ge et al., 2006;
Harland et al., 2002; Siegel et al., 1992). In
addition, this result supports prior work related
to ACEs and cumulative risk, in that the more
SLEs a person and/or family experiences, the
higher likelihood they have of experiencing
negative health and well-being outcomes (Fe-
litti, 1993, 2019; Felitti et al., 2019).
Additionally, results showed that health-
related SLEs were associated with higher levels
of emotional and behavioral problems in all
children. Thus, it may be the case that certain
SLEs can have a stronger influence on chil-
dren’s emotional and behavioral well-being
than other SLEs. Furthermore, when examined
by race and ethnicity, Somali children had the
highest levels of all behavioral and emotional
problems across all SLEs. Overall, these results
suggest that children are experiencing higher
levels of emotional and behavioral problems in
the face of SLEs and that Somali children may
be at highest risk. It is important for future
research to both further examine this relation-
ship to identify potential protective factors for
prevention as well as to identify resources to
help with treatment for children. Regarding So-
mali children, it may be important for qualita-
tive research to be conducted to better under-
stand how Somali families experience SLEs and
how they perceive their family functioning, in
addition to their child’s emotional and behav-
ioral well-being, in the face of SLEs.
Regarding family functioning, there was no
significant association between total SLEs and
overall family functioning; however, when cer-
tain SLEs occurred (e.g., health-related, legal),
391STRESSFUL LIFE EVENTS IN DIVERSE FAMILIES
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
Ps
yc
ho
lo
gi
ca
l
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
on
e
of
its
al
lie
d
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
they were associated with lower family func-
tioning within racially/ethnically diverse sub-
populations. This supports prior studies show-
ing SLEs are associated with lower family
functioning and extends prior research by show-
ing there may be differences by race or ethnicity
(Thoits, 2010). Thus, it may be the case that
specific SLEs are harder to cope with than oth-
ers for particular populations. For example, So-
mali families reported lower family functioning
in the face of health-related and legal SLEs.
There may also be differential implications for
certain types of SLEs for different racial or
ethnic groups (e.g., legal issues may have dif-
ferent implications or be of a different type on
average for Latino families vs. Somali fami-
lies).
Overall, findings from the current study un-
derscore the importance of applying critical race
theory to the understanding of physical, emo-
tional, and behavioral health concerns in chil-
dren and families and for discussing race and
immigrant and refugee status in the context of
assessing SLEs (Cunningham, 2014; Cunning-
ham & Scarlato, 2018). Many important factors,
such as discrimination or harassment, structural
racism, citizenship experience, reason or story
for leaving country of origin, and trauma or
torture, may influence study findings. It is im-
portant for future research, especially qualita-
tive, to further investigate these findings to in-
form interventions and implications for
practitioners.
Strengths and Limitations
This study had both strengths and weak-
nesses. A marked strength of the study is the
diversity of the sample population, which in-
cluded racially and ethnically diverse, immi-
grant and refugee, and low-income families.
Examining both individual child-level emo-
tional and behavioral well-being and familial
well-being was another strength of the study. In
addition, the ability to control for multiple po-
tential confounders (i.e., parent gender, weight,
education, income, mental health (depression,
anxiety), number of children in the household,
and time in the United States) is a further
strength. One limitation of this study is that
other confounding variables related to child and
family functioning that can vary between
groups by citizenship experience or immigra-
tion status, such as voluntarily leaving one’s
country of origin versus fleeing for safety and
access to community services or resources,
were not assessed and may change the findings
in this study; these variables should be studied
in future research. Another limitation of the
study is the overall small sample size (n � 150)
and even smaller sample size when examining
associations by each racial and ethnic group
(n � 25 per group). However, given SLEs and
mental health outcomes have not been studied
in such diverse populations before, including
immigrant and refugee households, this study
provides an initial important examination of
these factors that future larger studies can use as
a starting point to verify results and expand
findings. In addition, the cross-sectional nature
of the study limited our ability to understand
temporality of the associations. Furthermore,
this study relied on parent report of SLEs and
child emotional and behavioral problems, which
may have introduced some social desirability
bias. The measure of SLEs was also limited to
the prior 6 months, and it may be important to
assess a longer time period of exposure to SLEs,
such as 12 months. The SLE measure, Strengths
and Difficulties Questionnaire, and family func-
tioning measure are also limited by being vali-
dated on only some of the populations we stud-
ied. It may also be possible that families who
are immigrants or refugees have difficulty
understanding the way survey questions are
asked or there may not be words for survey
items that are easily translatable into their
language; however, all survey materials were
translated into Spanish, Hmong, and Somali
by individuals from their own local commu-
nities. Additionally, immigrant or refugee
families come from a variety of contexts, and
it may be the case that families from war-torn
countries may report and/or experience SLEs
differently from families from other countries
and backgrounds.
Conclusion
Results of the current study showed that all
families in this racially and ethnically diverse
and immigrant and refugee sample were expe-
riencing SLEs. In addition, the majority of di-
verse children were experiencing emotional and
behavioral problems (i.e., hyperactivity, emo-
tional problems) in the face of SLEs (i.e., com-
392 BERGE ET AL.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
Ps
yc
ho
lo
gi
ca
l
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
on
e
of
its
al
lie
d
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
bined SLE score, health-related events), with
Somali children being at highest risk. Addition-
ally, the majority of diverse families did not
experience lower family functioning in re-
sponse the SLEs, except regarding certain SLEs
(i.e., health-related, legal). However, specific
families (i.e., Somali) experienced lower family
functioning in the face of the majority of SLEs.
These results suggest implications for practitio-
ners (mental health and health care providers)
working with diverse families regarding SLEs.
For example, it would be important for practi-
tioners to know that the majority of families
experience one or two SLEs and that given the
cumulative nature of SLEs it would be impor-
tant to provide families with resources to min-
imize the stress produced by these events. In
addition, practitioners should potentially con-
sider screening for SLEs and providing extra
resources for children, given all children in the
study had some emotional and behavioral prob-
lems in the face of SLEs and the impact of
cumulative risk with SLEs (Felitti, 1993, 2019;
Felitti et al., 2019). Additionally, it would be
important for practitioners to know which fam-
ilies are at greatest risk for experiencing SLEs
(i.e., African American, Native American, So-
mali families) to ensure they are provided with
the resources necessary to mitigate the impact
of SLEs. Practitioners may also want to utilize
family-level resources, given results of this
study showed most diverse families had high
family functioning in the face of SLEs. Further-
more, practitioners working with Somali fami-
lies may want to provide resources for both
family functioning and child emotional and be-
havioral problems when SLEs are experienced.
References
Abraído-Lanza, A. F., Armbrister, A. N., Flórez,
K. R., & Aguirre, A. N. (2006). Toward a theory-
driven model of acculturation in public health re-
search. American Journal of Public Health, 96,
1342–1346. http://dx.doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2005
.064980
Allen, J. L., Rapee, R. M., & Sandberg, S. (2008).
Severe life events and chronic adversities as ante-
cedents to anxiety in children: A matched control
study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36,
1047–1056. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-008-
9240-x
Arcan, C., Larson, N., Bauer, K., Berge, J., Story, M.,
& Neumark-Sztainer, D. (2014). Dietary and
weight-related behaviors and body mass index
among Hispanic, Hmong, Somali, and White ado-
lescents. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and
Dietetics, 114, 375–383. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.jand.2013.11.019
Berge, J. M., Loth, K., Hanson, C., Croll-Lampert, J.,
& Neumark-Sztainer, D. (2012). Family life cycle
transitions and the onset of eating disorders: A
retrospective grounded theory approach. Journal
of Clinical Nursing, 21, 1355–1363. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1111/j.1365-2702.2011.03762.x
Berge, J. M., Trofholz, A., Tate, A. D., Beebe, M.,
Fertig, A., Miner, M. H., . . . Neumark-Sztainer, D.
(2017). Examining unanswered questions about
the home environment and childhood obesity dis-
parities using an incremental, mixed-methods, lon-
gitudinal study design: The Family Matters study.
Contemporary Clinical Trials, 62, 61–76. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1016/j.cct.2017.08.002
Brugha, T. S., & Cragg, D. (1990). The List of
Threatening Experiences: The reliability and va-
lidity of a brief life events questionnaire. Acta
Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 82, 77–81. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.1990.tb01360.x
Byles, J., Byrne, C., Boyle, M. H., & Offord, D. R.
(1988). Ontario Child Health Study: Reliability
and validity of the general functioning subscale of
the McMaster Family Assessment Device. Family
Process, 27, 97–104. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j
.1545-5300.1988.00097.x
Conger, R. D., Wallace, L. E., Sun, Y., Simons, R. L.,
McLoyd, V. C., & Brody, G. H. (2002). Economic
pressure in African American families: A replica-
tion and extension of the family stress model.
Developmental Psychology, 38, 179–193. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.2.179
Cooper, R., Cutler, J., Desvigne-Nickens, P., Fort-
mann, S. P., Friedman, L., Havlik, R., . . . Thom,
T. (2000). Trends and disparities in coronary heart
disease, stroke, and other cardiovascular diseases
in the United States: Findings of the National
Conference on Cardiovascular Disease Prevention.
Circulation, 102, 3137–3147. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1161/01.CIR.102.25.3137
Cronholm, P. F., Forke, C. M., Wade, R., Bair-
Merritt, M. H., Davis, M., Harkins-Schwarz, M.,
. . . Fein, J. A. (2015). Adverse childhood experi-
ences: Expanding the concept of adversity. Amer-
ican Journal of Preventive Medicine, 49, 354–361.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2015.02.001
Cunningham, B. A. (2014). Race: A starting place.
Virtual Mentor, 16, 472–478. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1001/virtualmentor.2014.16.6.msoc1-1406
Cunningham, B. A., & Scarlato, A. S. M. (2018).
Ensnared by colorblindness: Discourse on health
care disparities. Ethnicity & Disease, 28(Suppl. 1),
235–240. http://dx.doi.org/10.18865/ed.28.S1.235
393STRESSFUL LIFE EVENTS IN DIVERSE FAMILIES
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
Ps
yc
ho
lo
gi
ca
l
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
on
e
of
its
al
lie
d
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2005.064980
http://dx.doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2005.064980
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-008-9240-x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-008-9240-x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jand.2013.11.019
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jand.2013.11.019
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2702.2011.03762.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2702.2011.03762.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cct.2017.08.002
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cct.2017.08.002
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.1990.tb01360.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.1990.tb01360.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1545-5300.1988.00097.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1545-5300.1988.00097.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.2.179
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.2.179
http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/01.CIR.102.25.3137
http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/01.CIR.102.25.3137
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2015.02.001
http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/virtualmentor.2014.16.6.msoc1-1406
http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/virtualmentor.2014.16.6.msoc1-1406
http://dx.doi.org/10.18865/ed.28.S1.235
Ellis, B. H., MacDonald, H. Z., Lincoln, A. K., &
Cabral, H. J. (2008). Mental health of Somali
adolescent refugees: The role of trauma, stress, and
perceived discrimination. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 76, 184–193. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.76.2.184
Escobar, J. I., Hoyos Nervi, C., & Gara, M. A.
(2000). Immigration and mental health: Mexican
Americans in the United States. Harvard Review of
Psychiatry, 8, 64–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
hrp_8.2.64
Felitti, V. J. (1993). Childhood sexual abuse, depres-
sion, and family dysfunction in adult obese pa-
tients: A case control study. Southern Medical
Journal, 86, 732–736. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/
00007611-199307000-00002
Felitti, V. J. (2019). Origins of the ACE Study. Amer-
ican Journal of Preventive Medicine, 56, 787–789.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2019.02.011
Felitti, V. J., Anda, R. F., Nordenberg, D., William-
son, D. F., Spitz, A. M., Edwards, V., . . . Marks,
J. S. (2019). Reprint of: Relationship of childhood
abuse and household dysfunction to many of the
leading causes of death in adults: The Adverse
Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. American
Journal of Preventive Medicine, 56, 774–786.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2019.04.001
Ge, X., Natsuaki, M. N., & Conger, R. D. (2006).
Trajectories of depressive symptoms and stressful
life events among male and female adolescents in
divorced and nondivorced families. Development
and Psychopathology, 18, 253–273. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1017/S0954579406060147
Gee, G. C., Ryan, A., Laflamme, D. J., & Holt, J.
(2006). Self-reported discrimination and mental
health status among African descendants, Mexican
Americans, and other Latinos in the New Hamp-
shire REACH 2010 Initiative: The added dimen-
sion of immigration. American Journal of Public
Health, 96, 1821–1828. http://dx.doi.org/10.2105/
AJPH.2005.080085
Goodman, R. (2001). Psychometric properties of the
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. Journal
of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry, 40, 1337–1345. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1097/00004583-200111000-00015
Grant, B. F., Stinson, F. S., Hasin, D. S., Dawson,
D. A., Chou, S. P., & Anderson, K. (2004). Immi-
gration and lifetime prevalence of DSM–IV psy-
chiatric disorders among Mexican Americans and
non-Hispanic Whites in the United States: Results
from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alco-
hol and Related Conditions. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 61, 1226–1233. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1001/archpsyc.61.12.1226
Guo, J. (2016, September 14). Donald Trump might
be causing a major shift in how young Americans
feel about immigrants. Washington Post. Retrieved
from https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/wonk/
wp/2016/09/14/donald-trump-might-be-causing-a-
major-shift-in-how-young-americans-feel-about-
immigrants/?utm_term�.8755aabe4629
Harland, P., Reijneveld, S. A., Brugman, E., Ver-
loove-Vanhorick, S. P., & Verhulst, F. C. (2002).
Family factors and life events as risk factors for
behavioural and emotional problems in children.
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 11,
176–184. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00787-002-
0277-z
Hoyt, L. T., Zeiders, K. H., Chaku, N., Toomey,
R. B., & Nair, R. L. (2018). Young adults’ psy-
chological and physiological reactions to the 2016
U.S. presidential election. Psychoneuroendocri-
nology, 92, 162–169. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
.psyneuen.2018.03.011
Kaczmarek, M., & Trambacz-Oleszak, S. (2017).
HRQoL impact of stressful life events in children
beginning primary school: Results of a prospective
study in Poland. Quality of Life Research, 26,
95–106. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11136-016-
1371-x
Krieger, N., Kosheleva, A., Waterman, P. D., Chen,
J. T., & Koenen, K. (2011). Racial discrimination,
psychological distress, and self-rated health among
U.S.-born and foreign-born Black Americans.
American Journal of Public Health, 101, 1704–
1713. http://dx.doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2011
.300168
Lee, S., & Chang, J. (2012). Mental health status of
the Hmong Americans in 2011: Three decades
revisited. Journal of Social Work in Disability &
Rehabilitation, 11, 55–70. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1080/1536710X.2012.648117
Loth, K., van den Berg, P., Eisenberg, M. E., &
Neumark-Sztainer, D. (2008). Stressful life events
and disordered eating behaviors: Findings from
Project EAT. Journal of Adolescent Health, 43,
514–516. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth
.2008.03.007
Manyema, M., Norris, S. A., & Richter, L. M. (2018).
Stress begets stress: The association of adverse
childhood experiences with psychological distress
in the presence of adult life stress. BMC Public
Health, 18, 835. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12889-
018-5767-0
Mensah, G. A., Mokdad, A. H., Ford, E. S., Green-
lund, K. J., & Croft, J. B. (2005). State of dispar-
ities in cardiovascular health in the United States.
Circulation, 111, 1233–1241. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1161/01.CIR.0000158136.76824.04
Neece, C. L., Green, S. A., & Baker, B. L. (2012).
Parenting stress and child behavior problems: A
transactional relationship across time. American
Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Dis-
abilities, 117, 48–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1352/
1944-7558-117.1.48
394 BERGE ET AL.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
Ps
yc
ho
lo
gi
ca
l
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
on
e
of
its
al
lie
d
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.76.2.184
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.76.2.184
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/hrp_8.2.64
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/hrp_8.2.64
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00007611-199307000-00002
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00007611-199307000-00002
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2019.02.011
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2019.04.001
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579406060147
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579406060147
http://dx.doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2005.080085
http://dx.doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2005.080085
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200111000-00015
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200111000-00015
http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.61.12.1226
http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.61.12.1226
https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/wonk/wp/2016/09/14/donald-trump-might-be-causing-a-major-shift-in-how-young-americans-feel-about-immigrants/?utm_term=.8755aabe4629
https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/wonk/wp/2016/09/14/donald-trump-might-be-causing-a-major-shift-in-how-young-americans-feel-about-immigrants/?utm_term=.8755aabe4629
https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/wonk/wp/2016/09/14/donald-trump-might-be-causing-a-major-shift-in-how-young-americans-feel-about-immigrants/?utm_term=.8755aabe4629
https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/wonk/wp/2016/09/14/donald-trump-might-be-causing-a-major-shift-in-how-young-americans-feel-about-immigrants/?utm_term=.8755aabe4629
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00787-002-0277-z
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00787-002-0277-z
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.03.011
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.03.011
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11136-016-1371-x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11136-016-1371-x
http://dx.doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2011.300168
http://dx.doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2011.300168
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1536710X.2012.648117
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1536710X.2012.648117
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2008.03.007
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2008.03.007
http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12889-018-5767-0
http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12889-018-5767-0
http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/01.CIR.0000158136.76824.04
http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/01.CIR.0000158136.76824.04
http://dx.doi.org/10.1352/1944-7558-117.1.48
http://dx.doi.org/10.1352/1944-7558-117.1.48
Neumark-Sztainer, D., Croll, J., Story, M., Hannan,
P. J., French, S. A., & Perry, C. (2002). Ethnic/
racial differences in weight-related concerns and
behaviors among adolescent girls and boys: Find-
ings from Project EAT. Journal of Psychosomatic
Research, 53, 963–974. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
S0022-3999(02)00486-5
Ortega, A. N., Rosenheck, R., Alegría, M., & Desai,
R. A. (2000). Acculturation and the lifetime risk of
psychiatric and substance use disorders among
Hispanics. Journal of Nervous and Mental Dis-
ease, 188, 728–735. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/
00005053-200011000-00002
Siddiqui, S. (2016, January 27). Trump signs “ex-
treme vetting” executive order for people entering
the U.S. The Guardian. Retrieved from https://
www.theguardian.com/us-news/2017/jan/27/
donald-trump-muslim-refugee-ban-executive-
action
Siegel, K., Mesagno, F. P., Karus, D., Christ, G.,
Banks, K., & Moynihan, R. (1992). Psychosocial
adjustment of children with a terminally ill parent.
Journal of the American Academy of Child &
Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 327–333. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1097/00004583-199203000-00022
Thoits, P. A. (2010). Stress and health: Major find-
ings and policy implications. Journal of Health
and Social Behavior, 51(Suppl.), S41–S53. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022146510383499
van den Berg, P. A., Mond, J., Eisenberg, M., Ack-
ard, D., & Neumark-Sztainer, D. (2010). The link
between body dissatisfaction and self-esteem in
adolescents: Similarities across gender, age,
weight status, race/ethnicity, and socioeconomic
status. Journal of Adolescent Health, 47, 290–296.
Whitchurch, G. G., & Constantine, L. L. (1993).
Systems theory. In P. G. Boss, W. J. Doherty, R.
LaRossa, W. R. Schumm, & S. K. Steinmetz
(Eds.), Sourcebook on family theories and meth-
ods: A contextual approach (pp. 325–355). http://
dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-85764-0_14
White, J., Klein, D., & Martin, T. (2015). Family
theories: An introduction (4th ed.). Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Williams, D. R., & Medlock, M. M. (2017). Health
effects of dramatic societal events—Ramification
of the recent presidential elections. New England
Journal of Medicine, 376, 2295–2299. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1056/NEJMms1702111
Received January 16, 2020
Revision received April 28, 2020
Accepted July 5, 2020 �
395STRESSFUL LIFE EVENTS IN DIVERSE FAMILIES
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
Ps
yc
ho
lo
gi
ca
l
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
on
e
of
its
al
lie
d
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3999%2802%2900486-5
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3999%2802%2900486-5
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00005053-200011000-00002
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00005053-200011000-00002
https://www.theguardian.com/us-news/2017/jan/27/donald-trump-muslim-refugee-ban-executive-action
https://www.theguardian.com/us-news/2017/jan/27/donald-trump-muslim-refugee-ban-executive-action
https://www.theguardian.com/us-news/2017/jan/27/donald-trump-muslim-refugee-ban-executive-action
https://www.theguardian.com/us-news/2017/jan/27/donald-trump-muslim-refugee-ban-executive-action
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199203000-00022
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199203000-00022
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022146510383499
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022146510383499
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-85764-0_14
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-85764-0_14
http://dx.doi.org/10.1056/NEJMms1702111
http://dx.doi.org/10.1056/NEJMms1702111
Stressful Life Events and Associations With Child and Family Emotional and Behavioral Well-Being …
Method
Participants
Procedures and Data Collection
Measures
Statistical Analysis
Results
Discussion
Strengths and Limitations
Conclusion
References